Skip to main content

Brain regions essential for improved lexical access in an aged aphasic patient: a case report

Abstract

Background

The relationship between functional recovery after brain injury and concomitant neuroplastic changes is emphasized in recent research. In the present study we aimed to delineate brain regions essential for language performance in aphasia using functional magnetic resonance imaging and acquisition in a temporal sparse sampling procedure, which allows monitoring of overt verbal responses during scanning.

Case presentation

An 80-year old patient with chronic aphasia (2 years post-onset) was investigated before and after intensive language training using an overt picture naming task. Differential brain activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus for correct word retrieval and errors was found. Improved language performance following therapy was mirrored by increased fronto-thalamic activation while stability in more general measures of attention/concentration and working memory was assured. Three healthy age-matched control subjects did not show behavioral changes or increased activation when tested repeatedly within the same 2-week time interval.

Conclusion

The results bear significance in that the changes in brain activation reported can unequivocally be attributed to the short-term training program and a language domain-specific plasticity process. Moreover, it further challenges the claim of a limited recovery potential in chronic aphasia, even at very old age. Delineation of brain regions essential for performance on a single case basis might have major implications for treatment using transcranial magnetic stimulation.

Peer Review reports

Background

Aphasia, the loss of the ability to produce or comprehend language, constitutes a frequent consequence of left hemispheric stroke. Within the aphasic syndrome, one of the most frequent and disturbing syndromes is impaired lexical access (e.g. object naming). Even though patients may succeed in retrieving words, semantic paraphasias (misnaming) and neologisms (meaningless word creations) are frequently observed. Neurophysiological changes accompanying recovery of function in aphasia have remained controversial [1, 2] Even though recently two studies used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate overt language production in aphasia [3, 4] the neural substrate for lexical access and its different types of errors has never been investigated. Furthermore, recent efforts in aphasia treatment [5, 6] addressed naming performance by fascilitation or inhibition of brain regions using repetive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS). Therefore treatment may be improved if brain regions involved in successful and failing word retrieval can be distinguished.

In the present study, brain activation during word retrieval was examined by functional magnetic resonance imaging in an 80 year old female patient with with chronic aphasia (see methods). An overt-picture naming task was designed and a temporal sparse-sampling procedure [7] realized to assess the activity pattern correlated with correct responses and different error types. Such a design allows to monitor the overt verbal response during an off-phase of the scanner, while the hemodynamic response is acquired after a short time delay. Furthermore, post-hoc categorisation of different responses types (e.g. correct word retrieval, semantic paraphasias, neologisms) is permitted. In the present study patterns of brain activation were compared for correct responses and errors. Moreover, if there was a difference of activation for errors and correct word retrieval, this should be replicable by comparing a patient's erroneous responses before therapeutic intervention to responses to the same items named correctly after therapy.

Case presentation

We investigated an 80-year-old female patient with chronic Wernicke's aphasia (duration >2 years) following left hemispheric ischemic stroke (Fig. 1 shows the patient's lesion) of the middle cerebral artery who demonstrated a severe word retrieval deficit. The patient was tested before (T1) and after (T2) short-term intensive language training (Constraint-Induced Aphasia Therapy, CIAT [8, 9]), which comprises communicative language-games with a focus on word retrieval and is scheduled for three hours/day on ten consecutive days. Three healthy right-handed female controls (aged 76, 80, 85 years) underwent the same fMRI-paradigm twice within two weeks.

Figure 1
figure 1

The patient's lesion: The figure shows the normalised T1-weighted structural magnetic resonance scan. The left hemispheric lesion includes the inferior parietal lobe, the superior temporal lobe, postcentral regions, the rolandic operculum, parts of the insula and the inferior frontal gyrus (axial slices, left = left).

Methods

Language performance of the patient outside of the scanner was evaluated by a standardised language test (Aachen Aphasia Test, AAT [10]) and a naming test that included 150 items. Responses during the naming test were required without time restriction, while the first response of the patient was scored. To account for the potential improvement of working-memory functions and more general attentional processes after therapy two non-language tasks were administered: Visual working-memory was evaluated with the Benton Visual Retention Test [11]. More general attentional processes were assessed by a german non-verbal test of attention and concentration (Alterskonzentrationstest, AKT [12]), which constitutes an adaption of the D2 Test [13] and is specifically tailored to assess attentional capacities of the elderly or aphasic subjects.

MRI was performed on a 1.5 Tesla scanner (Philips Intera) at the Kliniken Schmieder in Allensbach. Functional MRI stimulation consisted of two alternating blocked conditions (overt picture naming/passively viewing a fixation cross; 5 consecutive stimuli for each condition, the sequence of picture-naming blocks was fixed at both sessions while picture presentation during trials was randomized). Pictures were taken from an internet database [14] and included 80 line drawings of objects, high name agreement scores for all stimuli was assured (average name agreement: 0.92 ± 0.09). Forty of these pictures were included in the therapy material and trained once daily. Stimuli were presented by a visor (VisuaStim, Resonance Technology, Inc.) for 3 seconds during which the overt verbal response had to be performed (responses were transmitted by a microphone, tape recorded and transcribed after scanning). After a short delay (0.8 sec.) a whole-brain functional MR volume was acquired (TR = 6 sec.; acquisition time TA = 2.2 sec.; 28 transversal slices, slice-thickness: 4.5 mm; resolution: 3.6 × 3.6 mm, Field of View = 230, acquisition matrix 64 × 64). Data was modeled using a finite impulse response function. A total of 160 functional whole brain volumes were acquired. No overt response was required for the control condition (fixation).

Functional MRI post-processsing was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM2, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK) and included standard slice-timing, realignment, normalization and smoothing (9 × 9 × 12 mm3 Gaussian Kernel) routines. Normalization in the patient included cost-function masking to prevent distortion of the image due to the lesion [15]. The design allowed post-hoc categorization of the patient's responses as correct naming, semantic paraphasia or neologism, rendering it effectively into an event-related design due to the temporally sparse imaging applied. To elucidate the differential neural contribution to correct word retrieval and error types a total of 26 correct responses, semantic paraphasias and neologisms were chosen from both sessions (13 from each session, response types were matched for length, frequency, naming agreement, visual complexity within and between response types). To assess the general activation pattern of correct responses and error types we calculated basic contrasts comparing correct responses and error types to rest (correct>fixation, semantic paraphasias>fixation, neologisms>fixation). To assess the differential neural substrate of correct responses and error types contrasts included correct responses versus semantic paraphasias (Correct>SP) and correct responses versus neologisms (Correct>Neo). Since the patient's performance improved after therapy we compared activation elicited by 10 selected trials that were categorized as errors (7 neologisms, 3 semantic paraphasias) before therapy to activation elicited by the same pictures after therapy, when the patient articulated a correct response. The general activation pattern elicited by naming in the control group was calculated as a fixed effects model (naming>fixation at T1). To assess changes of activation between sessions in the control group we compared naming before and after the two week time interval (T2>T1). Maximally activated voxels within significant clusters are reported. Anatomic localization was conducted using the Talairach Demon software [16].

Written informed-consent has been obtained from all participants. The study was approved by the local ethics committee and in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.

Results

We compared brain activation elicited by a total of 26 correct naming responses during the overt picture naming task to 26 semantic paraphasias and 26 neologisms. Comparison of correct responses and error types during fMRI yielded a differential activation pattern that was mainly characterised by increased activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) for correct words compared to paraphasias and neologisms (Figure 2a+b; Tab. 1, cluster threshold p < .05 corrected, k>50; voxel threshold p < .001, uncorrected; the general activation pattern for correct responses, paraphasias and neologisms is described in Tab. 2). Most interestingly, the difference between the activation pattern of correct responses and error types was more pronounced in the right IFG when responses were semantically less related to the target picture (Fig. 2a+b: words > semantic paraphasias > neologisms).

Figure 2
figure 2

Differential activation for correct and erroneous responses: Increased activation in the right IFG was found comparing correct word retrieval to (a) neologisms and (b) semantic paraphasias. (c) Correct word retrieval at T2, but not at T1 (the same pictures, N = 10) yielded increased activation in the right IFG, the right thalamus and right and left putamen (left = left, all contrasts: cluster threshold p < .05, k>50, FWE-corrected; voxel threshold p < .01, uncorrected).

Table 1 Differential brain activation of words and errors: Activation elicited by correct word-retrieval was compared to semantic paraphasias (words > semantic errors) and neologisms (words > neologisms). Furthermore, we compared activation elicited by erroneous responses (before therapy) to the same stimuli that were named correctly after therapy (correct trials > former errors; all contrasts: cluster threshold p < .05, k>50, FWE-corrected; voxel threshold p < .01, uncorrected).
Table 2 General activation pattern of the patient during picture naming for different response types. Contrasts included correct responses > fixation; semantic paraphasias > fixation; neologisms > fixation (cluster threshold p < .01, k>50, FWE-corrected; voxel threshold p < .001, uncorrected).

After a two week period of intensive language training [8] the patient improved significantly in the profile score of a standardised language test (Aachen Aphasia Test, AAT [10]), which is an index of the general aphasia severity, and the repeating subtest of the AAT. Naming performance during fMRI (pre/post: 13/23 correct responses; maximum 80) and the naming test outside of the scanner (pre/post 22/44; maximum 150) almost doubled. No improvement of visual working memory or attentional functions were observed (Tab. 3). Improvement of language performance was mirrored by changed brain activation in the fMRI naming task: when we compared correct responses after therapy that evolved from errors (thus, the same pictures before and after treatment; T1/T2) significantly increased activation was evident in the IFG, the right thalamus, right and left putamen and the anterior cingulate gyrus (cluster threshold p < .05 corrected, k>50; voxel threshold p < .01, uncorrected; Fig. 2c). Naming performance for controls was close to a ceiling effect at both sessions (mean T1/T2: 71/72 correct responses, Tab. 3). The fMRI activation pattern for naming compared to rest (fixation) in controls was comparable to that described in earlier studies using covert or overt picture naming tasks (for review [17]). Picture naming activated a large bilateral network that included frontal, temporal and occipital regions, the thalamus, basal ganglia and limbic structures (Tab. 4). The strongest change of activation in the control group was a decrease of activation in the left IFG at T2 (cluster threshold p < .01 corrected, k>50; voxel threshold p < .001), no increased activation was evident even at a lower threshold (voxel threshold p < .01).

Table 3 Test performance of the patient and 3 healthy age-matched female controls. The patient was investigated before and after intensive language therapy. For controls fMRI-scanning was repeated witin a 2-week interval.
Table 4 General activation pattern of the control group comparing naming with baseline activation (naming > fixation) at the first investigation (fixed effects model, N = 3; cluster threshold p < .01, k>50, FWE-corrected; voxel threshold p < .05 corrected).

Discussion

This case-study constitutes the first report to elucidate the neural substrate of correct word retrieval and error performance in aphasia. Differences between correct retrieval and errors were observed mainly in right inferior frontal areas. This was replicated after therapy: words correctly produced at T2, but erroneous at T1, exhibited increased activation in the right IFG and subcortical structures (right thalamus, right and left putamen). The thalamus seems crucially involved in semantic-lexical retrieval by mediating activity in frontal areas [18]. In the present case, this is supported by the gradually increasing involvement of IFG with increasing semantical relationship of responses to the target picture. Furthermore, effective compensatory activity of the right hemispheric IFG homologue during naming performance may depend on intact function of the left basal ganglia [6]. This would explain the increased activation in the right and left putamen that was associated with correct word retrieval after therapy.

The contribution of the left or right hemisphere to the recovery of language in aphasic patients following stroke is controversially discussed in the literature, in particular, activity in right hemispheric areas following stroke has been attributed to "...the loss of active inhibition or competitive interaction of the homologous (left frontal) area, or an inefficient dead-end strategy" [6, 19]. Several functional imaging studies on naming in aphasic patients showed no correlation between right hemispheric (frontal) activation and speech recovery [20–23], on the other hand, there are reports that emphasize the importance of right hemispheric areas to the recovery of function in aphasia [24]. For example, Winhuisen et al. [25, 26] showed that deactivation of the right IFG using rTMS interferred with performance during a semantic task in aphasic patients, indicating an essential contribution of this area to language recovery (at least in a subset of patients) and results of Crosson et al. [27] support the importance of right frontal areas in successful naming therapy.

In the present study we found gradually increased activation of right the IFG to be associated with better naming performance, which emphasizes the importance of this area for the recovery of naming performance in this particular patient. Still, language performance of the patient was low and even though improvements were substantiated, the overall naming capacity was still severely disturbed after therapy, which in some way points to a dysfunctional compensatory activation pattern. We can only speculate about the effect of rTMS to the right IFG in this patient and it might even be possible that inhibition of this area fascilitates naming performance. On the other hand, the results of the present case study show, that gradual involvement of the right IFG contributes to improved language functions, which makes the previous assumption unlikely.

We also substantiated additionally activated clusters in left perilesional (precentral) areas for correct responses compared to neologisms and sematic paraphasias (see Tab. 1), that may be functionally as important as the right hemispheric activation. However, in patients with primary progresssive aphasia [28] activation in precentral areas was negatively correlated with performance during a naming task using fMRI. The authors interpreted these results as "a compensatory spread of language-related neural activity or a failure to suppress activity in areas normally inhibited during language tasks". While to our knowledge no studies found associations between precentral activation and semantic tasks, activation of the right IFG has been implicated with semantic decisions by a number of studies [25, 26], which emphasizes its potential importance during recovery of naming perfomance in aphasic patients. Moreover, left precentral areas were not activated when we compared correct trials vs. former errors, which provides additional evidence for the importance of the right hemispheric IFG for improved task performance in this particular patient, rather than that of the left precentral activation pattern.

One critical issue in this case report is the relatively small number of improved responses during fMRI post-testing compared to the baseline. Still, naming performance was elicited under rigorous time constraints. Naming of the pictures had to be performed within three seconds. This might be an explanation for the relatively low number of improved namings. Moreover, taking into account the relatively low number of correctly named pictures at the first investigation (13/80) an increase by 10 correct namings equals an improvement of 77%. Furthermore, not only the number of correctly namind pictures increased, moreover, the number of neologisms halved (28/14) and the number of semantic paraphasias increased (i.e. there were more semantically related namings than before treatment). Therefore a closer look at the behavioral data during fMRI (obtained under rigorous time constraints) reveals an overall increase of semantically improved naming attempts after treatment. An additional naming test outside of the scanner without time restrictions was performed before and after treatment that included only untrained items. We also found an increase of correct naming responses of 83% (per/post: 24/44), when we also scored self-corrections, this increase was even more pronounced (31/63). Therefore, we are confident that there was an actual improvement of naming performance due to the treatment. This cannot merely be attributed to the training of specific items, rather the generalization to untrained items points to a general fascilitation of lexical access.

Some methodological issues need to be discussed: First, the most crital analysis (correct trials vs. former errors; Tab. 1) was based on 10 items, which is a comaparably week data basis. However, it has been shown convincingly that especially in temporal sparse sampling fewer trials are necessary to achieve the same power in brain activation [7]. With respect to that, we were optimistic to achieve a reliable activation pattern even with 10 trials per condition. In auditory fMRI, a recent study [29] replicated the now well-established finding of left anterior STS activation with intelligible over unintelligble speech material [30–32] on the basis of a mere 10 trials per condition. Therefore, we feel that it was both justified and conclusive to use even a comparably sparse data basis in performing this revealing contrast of error trials which have been corrected in post-testing.

Second, aphasic patients generally fluctuate in their language performance and multiple baseline investigations might be considered essential for the interpretation of therapy induced effects. In the present case report we did not assess the patient's performance during multiple baseline sessions for several reasons: First of all, the patient was in the late chronic stage of aphasia and major spontaneous improvement of language functions within short time intervals are not to be expected [33]. This would be different for the acute or post-acute stage, where spontaneous recovery and stronger fluctuations of behavioral performance would call for multiple baselines in any case.

Furthermore, there is evidence that repeated scanning produces stable activation patterns, even in severe chronic aphasia [34], in this study repeated scanning yielded a decrement of activation, supposedly related to task familiarity, rather than an increase of activation. Therefore, the increase of activation found in the present report is unlikely to be interpreted as the effect of the repeated measurement. From a more pragmatic viewpoint we chose not to asses stability in this 80-year old patient for the following reasons: Since we used the same pictures for pre-post assessment, repeated measures would have familiarized the patient with the test materials and an additional fMRI scan would have increased the stress upon the patient, this was especially important in this 80-year old lady. Furthermore, we included a gender and age-matched control group that evidenced no increased activation. This shows stability of the activation pattern induced by the paradigm across time.

Conclusion

We conclude that improvement in various language tasks is supported by improved communication within a larger functional cortical network that includes right hemispheric fronto-thalamic areas. This communication might be mediated by spared left basal ganglia functions. Moreover, the crucial impact of right hemispheric (frontal) areas for correct word retrieval evident in the present case supports the potential role of contralateral homologue brain areas in the recovery from aphasia [35].

The findings reported gain significance insofar as they reflect functional changes that are specific both to treatment and to the language domain: First, age-matched controls did not show changes in performance or increased brain activation, which allows to link changes observed in the patient to the treatment supplied. Second, the patient did not improve in less domain-specific measures of brain functioning: performance in both non-language specific tests did not improve after therapy. This ties the changes in brain activation even closer to therapy-induced recovery of language performance. Still, evidence suggests that there is considerable variablility in areas responsible for recovery of language functions in aphasia and the results of the present study are based on a single patient. Therefore, the findings from this case report might not generalize and replication in a larger sample is necessary.

Although based on data of a single patient, the global behavioral improvement after short-term intensive treatment [8, 9] challenges the claim of a limited recovery potential in chronic aphasia, even at very old age. This encourages further study of both therapy effectiveness and brain plasticity in aphasia-therapy, where elder patients form the rule rather than an exception. Furthermore, delineation of brain regions essential for language performance in aphasia on a single case basis, might be of major relevance to future treatment efforts using rTMS [5, 6].

References

  1. Price CJ, Crinion J: The latest on functional imaging studies of aphasic stroke. Curr Opin Neurol. 2005, 18: 429-434. 10.1097/01.wco.0000168081.76859.c1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Demonet JF, Thierry G, Cardebat D: Renewal of the neurophysiology of language: functional neuroimaging. Physiol Rev. 2005, 85: 49-95. 10.1152/physrev.00049.2003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Martin PI, Naeser MA, Doron KW, Bogdan A, Baker EH, Kurland J, Renshaw P, Yurgelun-Todd D: Overt naming in aphasia studied with a functional MRI hemodynamic delay design. Neuroimage. 2005, 28: 194-204. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Leger A, Demonet JF, Ruff S, Aithamon B, Touyeras B, Puel M, Boulanouar K, Cardebat D: Neural substrates of spoken language rehabilitation in an aphasic patient: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2002, 17: 174-183. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1238.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Naeser MA, Martin PI, Nicholas M, Baker EH, Seekins H, Helm-Estabrooks N, Cayer-Meade C, Kobayashi M, Theoret H, Fregni F, Tormos JM, Kurland J, Doron KW, Pascual-Leone A: Improved naming after TMS treatments in a chronic, global aphasia patient--case report. Neurocase. 2005, 11: 182-193.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Naeser MA, Martin PI, Nicholas M, Baker EH, Seekins H, Kobayashi M, Theoret H, Fregni F, Maria-Tormos J, Kurland J, Doron KW, Pascual-Leone A: Improved picture naming in chronic aphasia after TMS to part of right Broca's area: an open-protocol study. Brain Lang. 2005, 93: 95-105. 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.08.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Hall DA, Haggard MP, Akeroyd MA, Palmer AR, Summerfield AQ, Elliott MR, Gurney EM, Bowtell RW: "Sparse" temporal sampling in auditory fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999, 7: 213-223. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:3<213::AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-N.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Meinzer M, Djundja D, Barthel G, Elbert T, Rockstroh B: Long-Term Stability of Improved Language Functions in Chronic Aphasia After Constraint-Induced Aphasia Therapy. Stroke. 2005, 36: 1462 -11466. 10.1161/01.STR.0000169941.29831.2a.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Pulvermueller F, Neininger B, Elbert T, Mohr B, Rockstroh B, Koebbel P, Taub E: Constraint-induced therapy of chronic aphasia after stroke. Stroke. 2001, 32: 1621-1626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Huber H, Poek K, Weniger D, Willmes K: Aachener Aphasie Test. 1983, Göttingen, Hogrefe

    Google Scholar 

  11. Benton AL: The revised visual retention test. 1981, Bern, Huber

    Google Scholar 

  12. Gatterer G: Alters-Konzentrations-Test. 1990, Goettingen, Hogrefe

    Google Scholar 

  13. Brickenkamp R, Zillmer E: The D2-test of attention. 1994, Goettingen, Hogrefe

    Google Scholar 

  14. Szekely A, Jacobsen T, D'Amico S, Devescovi, A. A, E. HD, Lu CC, Pechmann T, Pleh C, Wicha N, Federmeier K, Gerdjikova I, Gutierrez G, Hung D, Hsu J, Iyer G, Kohnert K, Mehotcheva T, Orozco-Figueroa A, Tzeng A, Tzeng O, Arevalo A, Vargha A, Butler AC, Buffington R, Bates E: A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. Journal of memory and language. 2004, 51: 247-250. 10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Brett M, Leff AP, Rorden C, Ashburner J: Spatial normalization of brain images with focal lesions using cost function masking. Neuroimage. 2001, 14: 486-500. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0845.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, Kochunov PV, Nickerson D, Mikiten SA, Fox PT: Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Human Brain Mapping. 2000, 10: 120-131. 10.1002/1097-0193(200007)10:3<120::AID-HBM30>3.0.CO;2-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Murtha S, Chertkow H, Beauregard M, Evans A: The neural substrate of picture naming. J Cogn Neurosci. 1999, 11: 399-423. 10.1162/089892999563508.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Crosson B: Subcortical mechanisms in language: lexical-semantic mechanisms and the thalamus. Brain Cogn. 1999, 40: 414-438. 10.1006/brcg.1999.1088.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Belin P, Van Eeckhout P, Zilbovicius M, Remy P, Francois C, Guillaume S, Chain F, Rancurel G, Samson Y: Recovery from nonfluent aphasia after melodic intonation therapy: a PET study. Neurology. 1996, 47: 1504-1511.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Cao Y, Vikingstad EM, George KP, Johnson AF, Welch KM: Cortical language activation in stroke patients recovering from aphasia with functional MRI. Stroke. 1999, 30: 2331-2340.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Rosen HJ, Petersen SE, Linenweber MR, Snyder AZ, White DA, Chapman L, Dromerick AW, Fiez JA, Corbetta MD: Neural correlates of recovery from aphasia after damage to left inferior frontal cortex. Neurology. 2000, 55: 1883-1894.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Perani D, Cappa SF, Tettamanti M, Rosa M, Scifo P, Miozzo A, Basso A, Fazio F: A fMRI study of word retrieval in aphasia. Brain Lang. 2003, 85: 357-368. 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00561-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Zahn R, Drews E, Specht K, Kemeny S, Reith W, Willmes K, Schwarz M, Huber W: Recovery of semantic word processing in global aphasia: a functional MRI study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2004, 18: 322-336. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Musso M, Weiller C, Kiebel S, Muller SP, Bulau P, Rijntjes M: Training-induced brain plasticity in aphasia. Brain. 1999, 122 ( Pt 9): 1781-1790. 10.1093/brain/122.9.1781.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Winhuisen L, Thiel A, Schumacher B, Kessler J, Rudolf J, Haupt WF, Heiss WD: Role of the Contralateral Inferior Frontal Gyrus in Recovery of Language Function in Poststroke Aphasia. A Combined Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation and Positron Emission Tomography Study. Stroke. 2005

    Google Scholar 

  26. Blasi V, Young AC, Tansy AP, Petersen SE, Snyder AZ, Corbetta M: Word retrieval learning modulates right frontal cortex in patients with left frontal damage. Neuron. 2002, 36: 159-170. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00936-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Crosson B, Moore AB, Gopinath K, White KD, Wierenga CE, Gaiefsky ME, Fabrizio KS, Peck KK, Soltysik D, Milsted C, Briggs RW, Conway TW, Gonzalez Rothi LJ: Role of the right and left hemispheres in recovery of function during treatment of intention in aphasia. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005, 17: 392-406. 10.1162/0898929053279487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Sonty SP, Mesulam MM, Thompson CK, Johnson NA, Weintraub S, Parrish TB, Gitelman DR: Primary progressive aphasia: PPA and the language network. Ann Neurol. 2003, 53: 35-49. 10.1002/ana.10390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Narain C, Scott SK, Wise RJ, Rosen S, Leff A, Iversen SD, Matthews PM: Defining a left-lateralized response specific to intelligible speech using fMRI. Cereb Cortex. 2003, 13: 1362-1368. 10.1093/cercor/bhg083.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Scott SK, Blank CC, Rosen S, Wise RJ: Identification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left temporal lobe. Brain. 2000, 123 Pt 12: 2400-2406. 10.1093/brain/123.12.2400.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Bellgowan PS, Springer JA, Kaufman JN, Possing ET: Human temporal lobe activation by speech and nonspeech sounds. Cereb Cortex. 2000, 10: 512-528. 10.1093/cercor/10.5.512.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Davis MH, Johnsrude IS: Hierarchical processing in spoken language comprehension. J Neurosci. 2003, 23: 3423-3431.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Robey RR: A meta-analysis of clinical outcomes in the treatment of aphasia. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1998, 41: 172-187.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kurland J, Naeser MA, Baker EH, Doron K, Martin PI, Seekins HE, Bogdan A, Renshaw P, Yurgelun-Todd D: Test-retest reliability of fMRI during nonverbal semantic decisions in moderate-severe nonfluent aphasia patients. Behav Neurol. 2004, 15: 87-97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Crinion J, Price CJ: Right anterior superior temporal activation predicts auditory sentence comprehension following aphasic stroke. Brain. 2005, 128: 2858-2871. 10.1093/brain/awh659.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Talairach P, Tournoux J: A stereotactic coplanar atlas of the human brain. 1988, Stuttgart, Thieme

    Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank T. Elbert and C. Herbert for helpful comments on the final version of the draft. This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, Grant RO 805/11-4) and the Kuratorium Zentrales Nervensystem (ZNS, Grant 2001013).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marcus Meinzer.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they don't have any competing interests.

Authors' contributions

MM conceived and conducted the study, analysed the data and drafted the manuscript. TF, JO and RA were involved in the implementation of the fMRI design and set-up. TF and JO also provided their expertise in data analysis. DD and GB were responsible for diagnostic assessment and conducted therapy. BR, TF and JO participated in the discussion and final revision of the manuscript. MM, TF and JO revised the initial submission. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Meinzer, M., Flaisch, T., Obleser, J. et al. Brain regions essential for improved lexical access in an aged aphasic patient: a case report. BMC Neurol 6, 28 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-6-28

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-6-28

Keywords