Skip to main content

Midlateral medullary infarction presenting with isolated thermoanaesthesia: a case report

Abstract

Background

A small lateral medullary lesion could produce isolated impairment of temperature sensation without concomitant impaired pain sensation. However, only one such case has ever been reported, and there are no reports on subjective symptoms and detailed somatosensory testing.

Case presentation

Herein, we report the case of a 53-year-old female patient presenting with impaired temperature sensation on the left half of her body, from the neck down, following a small infarction of the right midlateral medulla. The chronological changes in the patient's introspection regarding impairment of thermoception and the results of detailed somatosensory tests, including thermal sense, are shown in this report.

Conclusions

Thorough somatosensory tests, personal descriptions of symptoms, and electrophysiological quantification of similar cases are needed to improve our understanding of the neurological separation of the sensations of pain and temperature at the medullary level.

Peer Review reports

Background

Infarctions limited to the lateral medulla can cause various combinations of cerebellar ataxia, vertigo, nystagmus, dysphagia, Horner syndrome, hoarseness, and impaired pain/thermal sensations. Of these symptoms, impaired pain and thermal sensations are the most common. The manifestations include ipsilateral trigeminal–contralateral limb/body, contralateral trigeminal–contralateral limb/body, or bilateral trigeminal–contralateral limb/body involvement; limb/body involvement without trigeminal involvement; or trigeminal involvement without limb/body involvement [1]. Impairment of pain and thermal sensations in the limbs and trunk can be caused by injury to the lateral spinothalamic tract, which runs through the midlateral medulla. On rare occasions, infarctions localised to this region can produce contralateral impairment of these two types of sensations independent of other symptoms [2, 3]. Results of cordotomy [4] and cases of central cord syndrome [5] indicate that at the spinal cord level, the two pathways are doubly dissociated and can be independently impaired. As a result, dissociation of these pathways in the medulla itself has been suggested [6]. To the best of our knowledge, there has only been one report of such a situation: a single case experienced by Arai et al. [7] where a patient presented with isolated impairment of thermoception following a small infarct of the midlateral medulla. However, we cannot know much about the symptoms in this case since the report does not provide details regarding the impairment of thermoception and the results of other somatosensory tests.

Herein, we report the case of a patient presenting with isolated impairment of thermoception following a small infarction of the midlateral medulla. Moreover, we present the patient’s introspective reports of the symptom, as well as detailed quantitative findings from somatosensory testing.

Case presentation

A 53-year-old right-handed woman with a history of smoking and hypertension complained of recurrent ‘coming-and-going’ headaches. After four days of consecutive headaches, she suddenly developed nausea and stagger. Moreover, upon washing her hands, she was completely unable to feel the coldness of the water on her left hand, and when getting into the bath, neither could she feel the water’s warmth on the left side of her body, from the neck down. Following these symptoms, she visited the hospital. Diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the head revealed a small high-intensity area in the midlateral portion of the right middle medulla (Fig. 1a); no abnormal findings were observed at any other location. The patient was thus admitted with a diagnosis of cerebral infarction, and therapy with aspirin was initiated at 100 mg per os once a day by the seventh day of hospitalisation, ozagrel sodium 40 mg intravenous twice a day by the fifth day of hospitalisation, and clopidogrel 75 mg per os once a day (continued after discharge). Her headache, nausea, and stagger disappeared by the fourth day of hospitalisation, but the thermoanaesthesia persisted, and the patient complained as follows: “When I touch the (steel) bed frame with my left hand, it does not feel cool,” and “When I put water in my hands to wash my face, only my left hand is unable to feel the coldness of the water. Both sides of my face are still able to feel it, so I think my face is okay.” According to the patient, she had never recognised any pain-related sensory abnormalities before or after hospitalisation.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance images at the time of admission revealing a small high-intensity area in the midlateral portion of the right middle medulla. b Three-dimensional computed-tomography angiography (3D-CTA) performed 10 days after admission showing stenosis in the right vertebral artery. c Fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) MRI conducted 14 days after admission showing a faint high-intensity area almost exactly coinciding with the high-intensity area observed in diffusion-weighted images at admission.

A neurological examination revealed impairment of thermoception in the left upper and lower limbs and trunk. No allodynia was present. However, the patient also reported paraesthesia, stating that “Cold things touching my left leg or trunk sometimes feel hot.” No other abnormalities, such as cerebellar ataxia, vertigo, nystagmus, dysphagia, Horner syndrome, hoarseness, impairment of pain sensation, or impaired facial somatosensory function, were present. Neuropsychological testing revealed no abnormalities related to linguistic function, general attention, general cognition, episodic memory, frontal-lobe function, or construction, and hemispatial neglect was absent (Table 1).

Table 1 Results of neuropsychological tests

Three-dimensional computed-tomography angiography (3D-CTA) performed 10 days after admission revealed stenosis in the right vertebral artery (Fig. 1b). Computed tomography (CT) of the cervical spine revealed no abnormalities. A fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) MRI performed 14 days after admission showed a faint high-intensity area almost coinciding with the high-intensity area observed in diffusion-weighted images acquired at admission (Fig. 1c). By this point, the patient’s impaired thermoception had subjectively subsided, and she instead reported a relative difference in the degree of thermoception on the left and right sides of her body, stating “When bathing, the water feels warm on the right side of my body, but not as much on the left,” and “When I touch my bed frame, even though I can somewhat feel its coldness with my left arm, the sensation is weaker than that on my right side.” We performed detailed testing of the patient’s basic and cortical somatosensory modalities [8] (Table 2) and measured somatosensory evoked potentials in both upper limbs.

Table 2 Somatosensory Test Results

The somatosensory tests were performed with shielding to blind the patient from the stimuli and stimulated body parts, with the patient’s hands manipulating objects. For basic somatosensory modalities, we tested pain sensation, temperature sensation, tactile sense, vibration sense, and joint position sense. Pain and thermoceptive tests were performed on the back of the hands and feet. To investigate pain thresholds, the force being applied to a pin was changed in 1 g increments to determine the minimum load that the patient reported as painful. The results were normal at 6 g for both the left and right upper and lower limbs. Then, while stimulating the left side with a pin with an 8 g load, we changed the load on the right side and asked the patient to report when she felt the same degree of pain as on the left side. The results at the back of the hands and feet were both equal at 8 g. To test thermoception, stimuli at different temperatures (40°C, 10°C, 50°C, and 0°C) were presented to the patient, and she was asked whether these stimuli felt hot, cold, noxiously hot, or noxiously cold, respectively. The patient responded affirmatively on both the right and left sides. However, upon investigating the threshold at which the patient felt a particular stimulus was warm by starting at 30°C and slowly increasing temperature, the results obtained at the back of the left hand and left foot were 5.6°C and 4.3°C higher than the corresponding right-side results, respectively, indicating a clear increase in the threshold. However, when using the same method but instead lowering the temperature to test the threshold for coldness, no clear left-right difference was observed at either the back of the hand or the back of the foot. Different temperatures were presented to the patient’s left side: 25°C as a mildly cold stimulus, 40°C as a warm stimulus, and 10°C as an obviously cold stimulus. Simultaneously, a stimulus starting at 30°C was presented to her right side, and based on her guidance, we sought to find the equivalent temperature on her right side based on her perceived temperature on the left side. We found that at a mildly cold temperature of 25°C, equal responses were observed at both the back of the hand and the back of the foot. However, when presented with a 40°C stimulus on the left side, the patient reported that the right-side stimulus felt identical at a temperature 7°C below that of the left when tested on the back of the right hand and 6°C below that of the left when tested on the back of the right foot. Further, when presented with a 10°C left-side stimulus, the patient reported that the right-side stimulus was identical at a temperature 8°C above that of the left when tested on the back of the right hand and 23°C above that of the left when tested on the back of the right foot. These results indicated that on her left side, the patient was perceiving warm things as considerably colder and cold things as considerably warmer than their actual temperatures. No left-right differences were observed for touch, vibration sense, or joint position sense. For cortical somatosensory modalities, we performed two-point discrimination tests at the middle fingertip and ball of the big toe on the left and right sides, as well as size and weight comparison tests and identification tests of geometric shapes and everyday items with the patient’s left and right hands. Our results indicated that all patient’s cortical somatosensory modalities were normal on both the left and right sides. The test methods for each sensation, other than pain and thermoception, are described in the footnotes of Table 2. In the somatosensory evoked potentials (Fig. 2) by medial nerve stimulation performed 13 days after admission, the N20 distal latency and P15-N20 amplitude were 18.7 ms/2.086 μV at C3’-A1 and 18.7 ms/2.586 μV at C4’-A2, respectively; none of these responses were abnormal.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Medial nerve somatosensory evoked potential tests indicating that the N20 distal latency and the P15-N20 amplitude were 18.7 msec/2.086 μV at C3’-A1 and 18.7 msec/2.586 μV at C4’-A2, respectively.

She was discharged home on the sixteenth day after admission. Her symptoms continued to improve, and at the outpatient visit 10 days after discharge, her only subjective symptom was dysesthesia, which was the feeling of warmth on her left trunk.

Discussion and conclusions

Based on the early symptom of headache and the findings of head MRI and 3D-CTA, a lateral medulla infarction associated with vertebral artery dissection was diagnosed in this case. In this case, the only neurological symptom was impaired thermoception except for the short-term headache and nausea and the stagger after onset. The lesion was confined to a small area in the midlateral portion of the right middle medulla. Accordingly, our findings are in good agreement with those of Arai et al. [7]. The result of somatosensory evoked potentials indicated that her lesion had not encompassed the medial lemniscus system. Therefore, this case supports Arai’s postulate [7] that only thermoception is impaired when a particular part of the lateral spinothalamic tract running through the midlateral medulla of the medulla oblongata is damaged.

From the patient’s introspective reports, we deduced how her subjective symptoms changed. At the onset, her subjective symptoms consisted of the inability to perceive either high or low temperatures. Two weeks later, her condition had improved to the point that her left side was only relatively worse at perceiving temperatures than her right, and after a month, all symptoms except dysesthesia had disappeared. This case demonstrates that in the case of small infarcts at this site, thermoceptual deficits may improve quickly. The precise reasons for improvement remain unknown. One reason could be that the lesion did not affect the entire thermoceptual pathway in the lateral spinothalamic tract. Another could be that the tissue damage was mild, as reflected in the faintness of the signal in the MRI FLAIR image conducted 14 days after admission.

We could only perform a detailed examination of somatosensory function by the time the patient’s impaired thermoception had improved to a relatively moderate level. However, at that time, we could confirm quantitatively or semi-quantitatively for almost all items that no abnormalities existed other than that in thermoception. In addition, we established that the patient’s sensory disturbance was not an absolute loss of sensations such as hot, cold, noxious hot, and noxious cold, but rather a decrease in sensitivity on the left side relative to the right. We could effectively and quantitatively confirm it matched the patient's introspection. All results obtained from the neuropsychological examination were normal. Therefore, it is unlikely that neuropsychological disorders influenced the patient’s introspective remarks or response to the somatosensory tests.

In this case, hot, cold, noxious hot, and noxious cold were correctly discriminated once the patient’s subjective symptoms had become a matter of relative strength/weakness of perception. Therefore, depending on the time after onset and the degree of disability, a routine neurological examination may not accurately identify damage to the thermoceptual pathway in the lateral spinothalamic tract. Detailed interviews and quantitative sensory tests considering the possibility of thermoceptual disorders are important to avoid overlooking this disorder in patients and correctly evaluate its degree.

This study is subject to some limitations. First, quantitative somatosensory tests were performed only after the patient’s subjective symptoms were largely alleviated. Without collecting data from he onset, we cannot realistically address which kind of introspection corresponds to what kind of test results. Second, our study cannot be used for future comparison since we did not use the standardised quantitative sensory testing developed by the German research network [10]. In the future, we hope that such a standard test will be performed sequentially from the time of onset for similar cases. In addition, our pain and thermoception examinations are based only on the patient’s subjective sensations and lack supporting electrophysiological data. Our observations would have provided further evidence if we performed blink reflex to infer on the region affected [11, 12] and the contact heat [5], cold [13], and laser evoked potentials [14] to discriminate the affected sensations.

Pain and thermoception disorders are doubly dissociated in the spinal cord [4, 5]. However, to the best of our knowledge, there are no case reports where only pain sensation was impaired at the medullary level while thermoception remained intact. The availability of such cases, and the necessary tests, would deepen our understanding of the neurological separation of these two sensations at the medullary level.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article is included within the article.

Abbreviations

3D-CTA:

Three-dimensional computed-tomography angiography

CT:

Computed tomography

FLAIR:

Fluid-attenuated inversion recovery

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

References

  1. Kim JS. Pure lateral medullary infarction: clinical-radiological correlation of 130 acute, consecutive patients. Brain. 2003;126:1864–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Blitshteyn S, Rubino FA. Pure sensory stroke as an isolated manifestation of the lateral medullary infarction. J Neuroimaging. 2005;15:82–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Kashiwamura Y, Kawai M, Ogasawara J, Koga M, Negoro K, Kanda T. Hyperalagesia with loss of temperature sensation in one side of the body due to pinpoint infarction of contralateral spinothalamic tract. Clin Neurol. 2009;49:262–6 (in Japanese).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Friehs GM, Schröttner O, Pendl G. Evidence for segregated pain and temperature conduction within the spinothalamic tract. J Neurosurg. 1995;83:8–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Rosner J, Hubli M, Hostettler P, Jutzeler CR, Kramer JLK, Curt A. Not hot, but sharp: Dissociation of pinprick and heat perception in snake eye appearance myelopathy. Front Neurol. 2018;21:1144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Suranyi L. Dissociation of spinothalamic sensation in lateral medullary infarction. Neurology. 1998;51:1773–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Arai M. Isolated thermoanesthesia associated with a midlateral medullary infarction. Neurology. 2002;58:169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Klingner CM, Witte OW. Somatosensory deficits. Handb Clin Neurol. 2018;151:185–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Bell-Krotoski JA, Fess EE, Figarola JH, Hiltz D. Threshold detection and Semmes–Weinstein monofilaments. J Hand Ther. 1995;8:155–62.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, et al. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006;123:231–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Cruccu G, Deuschl G. The clinical use of brainstem reflexes and hand-muscle reflexes. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;111:371–87.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Fitzek S, Fitzek C, Marx J, et al. Blink reflex R2 changes and localisation of lesions in the lower brainstem (Wallenberg's syndrome): an electrophysiological and MRI study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999;67:630–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Rosner J, Rinert J, Ernst M, Curt A, Hubli M. Cold evoked potentials: Acquisition from cervical dermatomes. Neurophysiol Clin. 2019;49:49–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Cruccu G, Iannetti GD, Agostino R, Romaniello A, Truini A, Manfredi M. Conduction velocity of the human spinothalamic tract as assessed by laser evoked potentials. Neuroreport. 2000;11:3029–32.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding

None.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KH1 acquired case data and drafted the manuscript. NK, RS, and DM acquired case data. KY and KH2 supervised the study and substantively revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Keisuke Hanada.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The patient provided written informed consent after receiving a detailed description of the study. This study was approved by the ethical committee of Yamagata Prefectural University of Health Science and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

The patient provided written informed consent for publication.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hanada, K., Yokoi, K., Kashida, N. et al. Midlateral medullary infarction presenting with isolated thermoanaesthesia: a case report. BMC Neurol 22, 268 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-022-02796-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-022-02796-x

Keywords