Skip to main content

The association of upper airway anatomy with cognitive test performance: the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis

Abstract

Background

Numerous upper airway anatomy characteristics are risk factors for sleep apnea, which affects 26% of older Americans, and more severe sleep apnea is associated with cognitive impairment. This study explores the pathophysiology and links between upper airway anatomy, sleep, and cognition.

Methods

Participants in the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis underwent an upper airway MRI, polysomnography to assess sleep measures including the apnea-hypopnea index (AHI) and completed the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI). Two model selection techniques selected from among 67 upper airway measures those that are most strongly associated with CASI score. The associations of selected upper airway measures with AHI, AHI with CASI score, and selected upper airway anatomy measures with CASI score, both alone and after adjustment for AHI, were assessed using linear regression.

Results

Soft palate volume, maxillary divergence, and upper facial height were significantly positively associated with higher CASI score, indicating better cognition. The coefficients were small, with a 1 standard deviation (SD) increase in these variables being associated with a 0.83, 0.75, and 0.70 point higher CASI score, respectively. Additional adjustment for AHI very slightly attenuated these associations. Larger soft palate volume was significantly associated with higher AHI (15% higher AHI (95% CI 2%,28%) per SD). Higher AHI was marginally associated with higher CASI score (0.43 (95% CI 0.01,0.85) per AHI doubling).

Conclusions

Three upper airway measures were weakly but significantly associated with higher global cognitive test performance. Sleep apnea did not appear to be the mechanism through which these upper airway and cognition associations were acting. Further research on the selected upper airway measures is recommended.

Peer Review reports

Background

About 25% of adults over the age of 50 in the US have cognitive impairment [1], and due to the large size of the affected population, understanding risk factors for this impairment is critical. An estimated 26% of Americans over age 30 have sleep apnea [2], and prior research suggests that more severe sleep apnea is associated with cognitive impairment [3]. Understanding the pathophysiology and links between upper airway anatomy, sleep measures, and cognition may help facilitate treatment and prevention of the adverse outcome of poorer cognitive function. Treatments can be tailored based on the associations discovered, such as oral appliances if craniofacial (bony) morphology is an important exposure and uvulopalatopharyngoplasty surgery if soft palate volume is a critical exposure [4].

Upper airway anatomy, including craniofacial and pharyngeal soft tissue structures, may be associated with cognitive function given the role of upper airway anatomy in sleep apnea risk, as sleep apnea is associated with impaired cognition [5,6,7,8,9]. The specific association between upper airway anatomy and cognitive function is unknown [5,6,7,8]. Numerous upper airway anatomy characteristics have been reported as risk factors for sleep apnea, and variation in these characteristics may help identify people with sleep apnea who have more severe downstream outcomes [5, 6]. Studies included in two meta-analyses did not all find the same upper airway measures to be associated with sleep apnea, but many of them found associations with greater soft palate and tongue area [5, 6].

Cognitive function is postulated to be negatively impacted by sleep apnea, and many studies find that sleep apnea is associated with poorer cognitive function in certain domains but not others [9]. One study did not find an association between the apnea-hypopnea index and eight different measures of cognitive function [10]. A large meta-analysis reported an increased risk of progressing to cognitive impairment and poorer executive function among people with sleep disordered breathing, but found no difference in memory or global cognition domains [9]. Accordingly, evidence is conflicting on whether and which aspects of cognitive function are impacted by sleep apnea. Due to these previously reported associations, we hypothesized that lower tongue volume and lower soft palate volume would be associated with better cognition.

Using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-based measurements of upper airway anatomy in the setting of a large, multi-ethnic, deeply phenotyped cohort study, this analysis assessed the association of upper airway measures with performance on four cognitive function tests that measure different domains of cognitive function. There are numerous measures of upper airway anatomy, and it is unknown which, if any, are associated with cognitive test performance. Therefore, we used model selection techniques to select from among over 60 upper airway measures those that are most strongly associated with the four cognitive function test scores. Exploring the association of selected upper airway anatomy measures with cognitive test performance, both alone and after adjustment for sleep study measures, may help elucidate whether and reasons why upper airway anatomy is a potential risk factor for the adverse outcome of cognitive impairment.

Methods

Study design/setting/subjects

The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA) enrolled a racially and ethnically diverse cohort of 6814 men and women from six areas of the United States, including Black, Chinese-American, Hispanic and White participants [11]. At baseline in 2000–2002, all participants were between 45 and 84 years old and had no history of cardiovascular disease.

Extensive data collection was done at baseline and included self-reported preferred language, education, and a blood draw with samples stored for later use and genotyping. Follow-up study visits have occurred every 2–6 years, including exam 5 in 2010–2012 and exam 6 in 2016–2018. In conjunction with exam 5 (2010–2013), 2261 MESA participants underwent a sleep assessment including in-home polysomnography, actigraphy, and a sleep questionnaire using the Women’s Health Initiative Insomnia Rating Index and Epworth Sleepiness Scale [12]. Measures of sleep extracted from the polysomnography results included sleep architecture, time in each sleep stage, and measures of nocturnal hypoxemia, most importantly the apnea-hypopnea index (AHI). For this analysis, AHI was defined as the number per hour of apneas or hypopneas associated with at least a 4% oxygen desaturation plus the number per hour of arousals.

At exam 6, a median of 6 years after the sleep study, a subset of participants included in the MESA Atrial Fibrillation ancillary study [13] completed four cognitive function tests: the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI, version 2), the Digit Symbol Coding Test, the Digit Span (DS) Forward and DS Backward tests [14]. The CASI consists of 22 items across nine cognitive function domains (“attention, concentration, orientation, short-term memory, long-term memory, language abilities, visual construction, list-generating fluency, abstraction, and judgment”) [15]. The CASI is administered over about 20 min and is scored from 0 to 100 with higher scores indicating higher cognitive function. Prior literature suggests CASI score cutoffs for dementia screening ranged from 71 to 86 depending upon the cultural and educational background of the test-takers [15]. This instrument can be used in people at all levels of cognitive functioning, is designed to be used in different cultural contexts, and was available in both Spanish and Chinese versions [15]. The CASI was selected as the primary outcome test for this analysis because it is a global measure of cognitive function. The Digit Symbol Coding, DS Forward, and DS Backward tests were secondary outcome measures. The Digit Symbol Coding test involves substituting symbols for numbers as quickly as possible over a span of two minutes [16]. This test measures one cognitive function domain, the complex attention domain, via the subdomains of sustained attention and processing speed [17]. The DS Forward Test, in which a person is asked to repeat lists of numbers of increasing length after hearing them, is a measure of complex attention [17, 18]. The DS Backward Test, in which a person repeats a list of numbers backward after hearing them, is a measure of the working memory subdomain of the executive function domain [17, 18]. Other key variables collected at exam 6, which were considered as potential confounders, included family income, alcohol use, and anthropometry measures including body mass index and waist circumference.

A median of 17 months after the exam 6 clinic visit, in 2018–2019, 1062 participants in the MESA Atrial Fibrillation ancillary study returned for an MRI of the brain and upper airway, conducted while the participant was awake [19]. To allow for fat quantitation, the upper airway MRI included a Dixon imaging sequence [20]. All subjects gave informed consent and institutional review boards at all MESA sites approved this study. The MRI images that contained the upper airway were examined by one experienced reader and both soft tissue (31 measurements) and craniofacial measurements (36 measurements) were calculated from each scan slice using an image analysis program [21]. Reproducibility of upper airway measurements using this method was very high, as most intraclass correlations were greater than 0.8 [20].

Statistical analysis – variable selection

We used lasso (Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator) regression to select reduced sets of measures most associated with cognitive function from among the 67 upper airway measures and 19 sleep study measures. Lasso regression is a data reduction machine learning technique that shrinks small coefficients to zero and thus removes from the model the measures least associated with the outcome [22]. Thus, lasso regression prevents overfitting and selecting variables that are not associated with the outcome, which can be a drawback of other machine learning methods like ridge regression or decision trees. Lasso performs well even when measures are highly correlated, though if a set of measures are highly correlated, lasso can randomly select one of the set, which can lead to variability of selected measures across imputations. The 19 sleep study measures we considered were selected based on their associations with cognitive function in previous reports [23, 24]. The complete lists of candidate measures are found in Supplemental Table 1. Skewed upper airway and sleep study measures were log base 2 transformed, and these included tongue fat volume and AHI. Participants missing data for a particular cognitive test outcome were excluded from models for that outcome. The median (interquartile range) number of missing upper airway and sleep study variables was 1 (IQR 0–2). Multiple imputation with chained equations (5 imputations) was used to account for missing exposure and covariate data.

We conducted the data reduction steps separately for upper airway measures and sleep study measures. For the upper airway measures, lasso models were used to select measures from the list of 67 candidate upper airway measures without inclusion of sleep study measures. Five-fold cross validation was used to select the lasso regularization parameter with the best model performance as measured by the mean squared error. Using this regularization parameter, the final set of upper airway measures was created by fitting a lasso model on the entire sample. All models were adjusted for the confounders of age, gender, race/ethnicity, study site, current alcohol use, education, income, language spoken, and APOE genotype, which are known to be associated with both the exposures and outcomes. Upper airway variables selected in more than half of the imputations were included in further models. The same process was repeated for the 19 sleep study measures without inclusion of the upper airway measures.

Another machine learning technique, Bayesian Model Averaging (BMA), was used as a confirmatory variable selection technique. BMA calculates the probability that each candidate exposure variable will be included in the best fitting model and can work well with correlated variables [25,26,27]. Like the lasso models, separate BMA analyses were run for the upper airway and sleep variable lists, and the same confounders were included in both BMA models. Variables with a greater than 50% average probability of being in the best fitting model across the five imputations were considered as selected by this technique. Results from the lasso and BMA analyses were considered together in selecting the lists of upper airway and sleep measures for the next step of the analysis [28].

Statistical analysis – variable association evaluation

The primary model coefficients and p-values were assessed by fitting four linear regression models using all five imputations including the selected upper airway measures, one for each of the four cognitive function tests as the outcome. The linear models were adjusted for the same covariates as the lasso and BMA models. To assess whether the association of upper airway anatomy with cognition is mediated by sleep study measures, another linear regression was run including the selected upper airway measures and the selected sleep study measures plus the standard sleep apnea defining variable, AHI, and the coefficients were compared between the reduced and expanded models. We conducted 2 sensitivity analyses. The first sensitivity analysis added adjustment for body mass index (BMI) and waist circumference to evaluate how much of the impact of upper airway measures remained after accounting for body size, and the second excluded the 2% of participants with a change of > 5 BMI units between the time of the sleep study and the upper airway MRI (between MESA exams 5 and 6). To assess possible effect modification, the primary models were stratified by sex and race/ethnicity and multiplicative interaction terms were used to determine statistical significance, with each racial/ethnic group compared against the other three groups combined.

To assess each step of the hypothesized causal pathway, we also examined the association of upper airway measurements with sleep apnea and the association of sleep measures with cognitive function. In a single multivariable model, the association of each of the selected upper airway measurements with sleep apnea as measured by the AHI was also assessed via linear regression. Using linear regression, the individual associations of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS), percentage of sleep time at less than 90% oxygen saturation, and AHI were examined in relation to the four cognitive function tests, as previously studied in MESA [24]. These models were adjusted for age, gender, race/ethnicity, study site, current alcohol use, education, income, language spoken, and APOE genotype. P-values less than 0.05 were considered significant, and all analyses were conducted in STATA 17 and R 4.1.2.

Results

From among the 3303 participants who attended exam 6, 591 with data on sleep study measurements, cognitive function, and upper airway anatomy were included in this analysis (Supplemental Fig. 1). The included participants had a similar sex and race/ethnicity composition as the 2712 participants not included, but were on average slightly younger, and had higher family income and higher education level (Supplemental Table 2).

Of the 591 participants included in the analysis, 54% were female, the mean age at exam 6 was 72 years, and 26% of participants were Black, 15% Chinese-American, 19% Hispanic, and 40% White (Table 1). A majority of participants (79%) had an AHI of 5 or more, and 41% had an AHI of 15 or more. The average and standard deviation of selected upper airway measure averages and the cognitive function test scores are displayed in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics of included participants: demographics, confounders, exposures, and outcomes

Association of upper airway variables with cognitive test scores

Specific upper airway anatomy measures were selected via lasso and BMA models as potentially having strong associations with CASI and DS Forward scores, but none were selected for the DS Backward or Digit Symbol Coding scores. For the CASI outcome, lasso models consistently selected soft palate volume, maxillary divergence (the angle formed by the incisors and the maxillary tuberosities), upper facial height (the distance from the top of the nose to the bottom of the nose) (Supplemental Figs. 2–4), and four other measures (mandible width at canine, retropalatal airway volume, retropalatal intermandibular volume, and retroglossal airway minimum anterior-posterior distance). The BMA models selected soft palate volume and maxillary divergence as most strongly associated with the outcome (Table 2). Upper facial height was close to being selected by the BMA models with probability of being in the best model of 43%. Accounting for the overlap in selected variables, soft palate volume, maxillary divergence, and upper facial height were considered the key measures going forward. No sleep study measures were selected by either the lasso or BMA models for the CASI.

Table 2 Selected upper airway and sleep variables in lasso and Bayesian Model Averaging models for associations with scores on four cognitive function tests

To obtain estimates of association coefficients and test for statistical significance for the machine learning selected variables, a linear regression model was used and found that larger soft palate volume, longer upper facial height, and greater maxillary divergence were significantly associated with higher CASI score indicating better cognition (Table 3). The coefficients were small, with a 1 standard deviation (SD) increase in these variables being associated with a 0.83, 0.75, and 0.70 point higher CASI score, respectively. The four upper airway variables selected by the lasso model but not by the BMA model were not significant in a linear regression analysis (Supplemental Table 3). Adjusting for AHI very slightly attenuated the association of soft palate volume with CASI score, from 0.83 to 0.76 (Table 3). Similarly, adjusting for BMI and waist circumference, and excluding participants with a > 5 BMI change (N = 11 excluded) from exam 5 to exam 6 very minimally attenuated these associations (Table 3).

Table 3 Multiple linear regression models for the association of the three upper airway variables selected by lasso and Bayesian Model Averaging models with CASI score (primary analysis), the primary analysis plus AHI, and two sensitivity analyses

In stratified linear regression analyses, different selected upper airway measurements were significantly associated with higher CASI score in the four racial/ethnic groups. In Black participants, soft palate volume was the only significantly associated upper airway variable and had a coefficient of 1.60 higher CASI score per SD higher soft palate volume. In Chinese-American participants, maxillary divergence was the only significantly associated upper airway variable and had a coefficient of 1.67 higher CASI score per SD higher maxillary divergence. In White participants upper facial height was the only significantly associated upper airway variable and had a coefficient of 1.43 higher CASI score per SD higher upper facial height (Table 4). None of the three upper airway variables were significantly associated with CASI score in the Hispanic participants. However, none of these differences by race/ethnicity were significant in tests for interaction (p > 0.1).

Table 4 In subgroups defined by race/ethnicity and sex, association of selected upper airway measures with CASI score using a single multiple linear regression model for each subgroup

In models stratified by sex, differences in the measures significantly associated with CASI were also seen. Soft palate volume (coefficient 1.78 per SD) and maxillary divergence (coefficient 1.13 per SD) were significantly positively associated with CASI score in the female participants, while the significant variable in the male participants was upper facial height (coefficient 0.83 per SD). Tests for interaction of these upper airway measures by sex were significant for soft palate volume (p = 0.006) but not for maxillary divergence or upper facial height (p > 0.5).

For the DS Forward outcome, tongue fat percentage and eight other upper airway measures were selected in 3 of 5 imputations via lasso models. Only tongue fat percentage was selected in BMA models, and accounting for the overlap in selected variables, only tongue fat percentage was considered the key measure going forward. Tongue fat percentage was not significantly associated with DS Forward score in linear models, as the difference in DS Forward score per doubling of tongue fat percentage was 1.04 (95% confidence interval − 0.10, 2.17). No upper airway measures were selected by the lasso or BMA models for the DS Backward or Digit Symbol Coding tests, and no sleep study measures were selected for any of the three secondary cognitive function outcomes (Table 2).

Association of upper airway variables with sleep apnea

Larger soft palate volume was significantly associated with higher AHI, while upper facial height and maxillary divergence were not significantly associated with AHI (Table 5). None of the selected upper airway variables were significantly associated with the Epworth Sleepiness Scale or the percentage of sleep time at less than 90% oxygen saturation.

Table 5 Single multiple linear regression model for the association of the three upper airway variables selected by lasso and Bayesian Model Averaging models with log2 transformed apnea-hypopnea index

Association of sleep study variables with cognitive test scores

In linear regression models, higher AHI was marginally associated with higher CASI score indicating better cognitive function (coefficient per AHI doubling 0.43 [0.01, 0.85], p = 0.04) while AHI, the Epworth Sleepiness Scale, and the percentage of sleep time with less than 90% oxygen saturation were not significantly associated with any of the other cognitive function tests.

Discussion

This exploratory analysis found that three upper airway anatomy measures were significantly but weakly associated with global cognitive function as measured by the CASI score: soft palate volume, maxillary divergence, and upper facial height. However, these three upper airway measures were not associated with the DS Forward, DS Backward, or Digit Symbol Coding test scores. In addition, the association with CASI score of one of the measures, soft palate volume, was in the opposite direction to our hypothesis. Also contrary to hypothesis, adjusting for sleep apnea severity as measured by the AHI did not attenuate the upper airway associations with cognitive test performance, and no measures of sleep, including measures of duration, fragmentation, or hypoxemia, were selected as significantly associated with cognitive function. Thus, the results do not support that the upper airway and cognitive function associations are working through the mechanism of sleep apnea-based hypoxemic damage to the brain, though it is possible that the key sleep measure mediator was not measured in this study. It is unclear through which biological pathways soft palate volume, maxillary divergence, and upper facial height are associated with cognitive function. Adjusting for body size via BMI and waist circumference did not attenuate the strength of these associations. Where fat is stored in the body has been shown to be associated with cognitive function [29]. It is possible that this study did not collect the most relevant fat measure or that higher soft palate volume may better capture relevant fat deposition than measures such as BMI or waist circumference. It is also possible that the identified upper airway measures may be markers for other unmeasured health conditions. These results do not suggest that clinical intervention on upper airway anatomy characteristics would provide benefits for cognitive functioning. The overlap in variables selected by lasso and BMA models, as well as previous studies reporting that the selected variables are associated with sleep apnea, provides reinforcement that the selected variables are plausible links between upper airway anatomy and cognitive function. These associations require additional investigation, as there are no previous studies of the association of upper airway measures with cognitive function test scores.

Regarding the association of upper airway dimensions with sleep apnea, our finding that larger soft palate volume was significantly associated with higher AHI matches what was found in previous studies. In two meta-analyses of the associations between lateral X-ray data and obstructive sleep apnea, both found that larger soft palate area, and larger tongue area were associated with obstructive sleep apnea [5, 6]. Additionally, a case-control study reported that larger soft palate length and larger hyoid-mandibular distance were associated with sleep apnea even after matching on BMI [30], and another study of obese persons reported that those with a higher AHI also had larger soft palate volumes and tongue volumes [31]. Two previous studies focused on tongue fat as an important upper airway anatomy parameter. The first found that compared to age, sex, race, and BMI matched controls, persons with sleep apnea had significantly larger tongue fat volumes [32]. In the second study, which examined weight change in persons with sleep apnea, tongue fat volume was longitudinally associated with AHI [20]. Based on these studies, we had hypothesized that lower tongue fat would be associated with better cognition, but the present analysis did not find tongue fat to be significantly associated with any of the cognitive function tests.

Regarding the association of sleep study measures with cognitive function, our results add to the mixed results in the existing literature. Contrary to our hypothesis, our analysis found that higher AHI was weakly associated with better CASI score. Two studies found no association between AHI and several measures of cognitive function [10, 33]. Nikodemova et al. found that higher severity of sleep disordered breathing was associated with poorer cognitive function in persons with an Alzheimer’s risk gene [3]. In 1752 MESA participants, Johnson et al. found that self-reported sleepiness and a higher percentage of sleep time with low blood oxygen, but not elevated AHI, were associated with lower DS Forward test scores, but not with CASI score [24]. Compared with the Johnson study, our analysis had a smaller sample size, higher average CASI score and lower median AHI. Additionally, the studies by Nikodemova and Johnson found the largest associations in APOE4 variant carriers, but our analysis did not focus on variant carriers and included too few APOE4 carriers to have sufficient power to find significant associations in this subgroup.

Several studies have reported differences by race in the association of upper airway anatomy with sleep apnea [21, 34, 35]. In this analysis, we examined differences by race/ethnicity in the association of upper airway anatomy with cognitive function. The results of this analysis suggested possible differences by race/ethnicity but these differences did not reach statistical significance, possibly due to lack of power. Differences by sex in the associations of upper airway anatomy with cognitive function were also found in this study, as larger soft palate volume was significantly associated with higher CASI score in women but not men. This difference by sex requires additional study. As with the overall results, these results do not support employing upper airway anatomy clinical interventions in subgroups of race/ethnicity or sex to improve cognitive functioning.

This exploratory analysis has a number of strengths, including a large sample size from a racially diverse cohort study, accurate and reproducible measurement of upper airway anatomy by MRI, and analyses utilizing two different machine learning techniques to perform variable selection. Limitations include lack of power within racial/ethnic groups, potential unmeasured residual confounding, potential selection bias of participants with higher cognitive function test scores and lower AHI, that sleep study measurements were taken about six years before the cognitive function measures, and that upper airway anatomy was measured about 17 months after cognitive function. While all study participants were untreated for sleep apnea at the time of the sleep study, sleep apnea treatment at the time of the cognitive function tests was not measured. Additionally, the assessed cognitive function tests are limited and do not measure all possible cognitive function domains.

Conclusions

Upper airway anatomy as measured by soft palate volume, maxillary divergence, and upper facial height was significantly associated with global cognitive test performance. The associations with CASI score were small in magnitude: each standard deviation difference in upper airway measures was associated with less than a point of difference in CASI score. By contrast, in this dataset, for each 10 years older age, CASI score was 2.2 points lower. Sleep study measurements did not appear to be the pathway through which these associations were acting. This study does not suggest that clinical intervention on upper airway anatomy characteristics would provide benefits for cognitive functioning. As these analyses were exploratory, further research focusing on the selected upper airway measures should be pursued.

Data Availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the MESA Coordinating Center but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study, and so are not publicly available. MESA data are however available from the MESA data coordinating center upon reasonable request and approval.

Abbreviations

AHI:

Apnea hypopnea index

CASI:

Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument

SD:

Standard deviation

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

MESA:

Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis

DS:

Digit span

Lasso:

Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator

IQR:

Interquartile range

BMA:

Bayesian Model Averaging

BMI:

Body mass index

ESS:

Epworth Sleepiness Scale

References

  1. Hale JM, Schneider DC, Gampe J, Mehta NK, Myrskyla M. Trends in the risk of cognitive impairment in the United States, 1996–2014. Epidemiology. 2020;31(5):745–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Peppard PE, Young T, Barnet JH, Palta M, Hagen EW, Hla KM. Increased prevalence of sleep-disordered breathing in adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(9):1006–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Nikodemova M, Finn L, Mignot E, Salzieder N, Peppard PE. Association of sleep disordered breathing and cognitive deficit in APOE epsilon4 carriers. Sleep. 2013;36(6):873–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Mansukhani MP, Olson EJ, Caples SM. Upper airway surgery for obstructive sleep apnea. JAMA. 2020;324(12):1161–2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Neelapu BC, Kharbanda OP, Sardana HK, Balachandran R, Sardana V, Kapoor P, et al. Craniofacial and upper airway morphology in adult obstructive sleep apnea patients: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cephalometric studies. Sleep Med Rev. 2017;31:79–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Armalaite J, Lopatiene K. Lateral teleradiography of the head as a diagnostic tool used to predict obstructive sleep apnea. Dentomaxillofac Radiol. 2016;45(1):20150085.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Nuckton TJ, Glidden DV, Browner WS, Claman DM. Physical examination: Mallampati score as an independent predictor of obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep. 2006;29(7):903–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Sutherland K, Keenan BT, Bittencourt L, Chen NH, Gislason T, Leinwand S, et al. A global comparison of anatomic risk factors and their relationship to obstructive sleep apnea severity in clinical samples. J Clin Sleep Med. 2019;15(4):629–39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Leng Y, McEvoy CT, Allen IE, Yaffe K. Association of sleep-disordered breathing with cognitive function and risk of cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74(10):1237–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Andre C, Rehel S, Kuhn E, Landeau B, Moulinet I, Touron E, et al. Association of sleep-disordered breathing with Alzheimer disease biomarkers in community-dwelling older adults: a secondary analysis of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Neurol. 2020;77(6):716–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bild DE, Bluemke DA, Burke GL, Detrano R, Diez Roux AV, Folsom AR, et al. Multi-ethnic study of Atherosclerosis: objectives and design. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;156(9):871–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Chen X, Wang R, Zee P, Lutsey PL, Javaheri S, Alcantara C, et al. Racial/Ethnic differences in sleep disturbances: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis (MESA). Sleep. 2015;38(6):877–88.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Heckbert SR, Austin TR, Jensen PN, Chen LY, Post WS, Floyd JS, et al. Differences by Race/Ethnicity in the prevalence of clinically detected and monitor-detected atrial fibrillation: MESA. Circ Arrhythm Electrophysiol. 2020;13(1):e007698.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Fitzpatrick AL, Rapp SR, Luchsinger J, Hill-Briggs F, Alonso A, Gottesman R, et al. Sociodemographic correlates of cognition in the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis (MESA). Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015;23(7):684–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Teng EL, Hasegawa K, Homma A, Imai Y, Larson E, Graves A, et al. The cognitive abilities screening instrument (CASI): a practical test for cross-cultural epidemiological studies of dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 1994;6(1):45–58.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Jaeger J. Digit symbol substitution test: the case for sensitivity over specificity in neuropsychological testing. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;38(5):513–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Wang Z, Dong B. Screening for cognitive impairment in geriatrics. Clin Geriatr Med. 2018;34(4):515–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Fink HA, Hemmy LS, MacDonald R, Carlyle MH, Olson CM, Dysken MW, et al. Cognitive outcomes after cardiovascular procedures in older adults: a systematic review 2014 October 27; 2020, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK285344/

  19. Austin TR, Nasrallah IM, Erus G, Desiderio LM, Chen LY, Greenland P, et al. Association of brain volumes and white matter injury with race, ethnicity, and cardiovascular risk factors: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. J Am Heart Assoc. 2022;11(7):e023159.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Wang SH, Keenan BT, Wiemken A, Zang Y, Staley B, Sarwer DB, et al. Effect of weight loss on upper airway anatomy and the apnea-hypopnea index. The importance of tongue fat. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2020;201(6):718–27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Xu L, Keenan BT, Wiemken AS, Chi L, Staley B, Wang Z, et al. Differences in three-dimensional upper airway anatomy between Asian and European patients with obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep. 2020;43(5). https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsz273.

  22. Muthukrishnan R, Rohini R. LASSO: a feature selection technique in predictive modeling for machine learning. 2016 IEEE International Conference on Advances in Computer Applications (ICACA). 2016;18–20. https://doi.org/10.1109/ICACA.2016.7887916.

  23. Djonlagic I, Mariani S, Fitzpatrick AL, Van Der Klei V, Johnson DA, Wood AC, et al. Macro and micro sleep architecture and cognitive performance in older adults. Nat Hum Behav. 2021;5(1):123–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Johnson DA, Lane J, Wang R, Reid M, Djonlagic I, Fitzpatrick AL, et al. Greater cognitive deficits with sleep-disordered breathing among individuals with genetic susceptibility to Alzheimer disease. The multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Ann Am Thorac Soc. 2017;14(11):1697–705.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Mu Y, See I, Edwards JR. Bayesian model averaging: improved variable selection for matched case-control studies. Epidemiol Biostat Public Health. 2019;16(2):e13048. https://doi.org/10.2427/13048.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Raftery A, Madigan D, Hoeting J. Bayesian model averaging for linear regression models. J Am Stat Assoc. 1997;92(437):179–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Raftery A. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociol Methodol. 1995;25:111–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Mera-Gaona M, Lopez D, Vargas-Canas R, Neumann U. Framework for the ensemble of feature selection methods. Appl Sci. 2021;11(8122). https://doi.org/10.3390/app11178122.

  29. Boccara E, Golan S, Beeri MS. The association between regional adiposity, cognitive function, and dementia-related brain changes: a systematic review. Front Med (Lausanne). 2023;10:1160426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Gupta A, Kumar R, Bhattacharya D, Thukral BB, Suri JC. Craniofacial and upper airway profile assessment in North Indian patients with obstructive sleep apnea. Lung India. 2019;36(2):94–101.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Turnbull CD, Wang SH, Manuel AR, Keenan BT, McIntyre AG, Schwab RJ, Stradling JR. Relationships between MRI fat distributions and sleep apnea and obesity hypoventilation syndrome in very obese patients. Sleep Breath. 2018;22(3):673–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kim AM, Keenan BT, Jackson N, Chan EL, Staley B, Poptani H, et al. Tongue fat and its relationship to obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep. 2014;37(10):1639–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Moon C, Melah KE, Johnson SC, Bratzke LC. Sleep-disordered breathing, brain volume, and cognition in older individuals with heart failure. Brain Behav. 2018;8(7):e01029.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Cakirer B, Hans MG, Graham G, Aylor J, Tishler PV, Redline S. The relationship between craniofacial morphology and obstructive sleep apnea in whites and in African-Americans. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2001;163(4):947–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Redline S, Tishler PV, Hans MG, Tosteson TD, Strohl KP, Spry K. Racial differences in sleep-disordered breathing in African-Americans and Caucasians. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1997;155(1):186–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the other investigators, the staff, and the participants of the MESA study for their valuable contributions. A full list of participating MESA investigators and institutions can be found at http://www.mesa-nhlbi.org.

Funding

This research was supported by contracts 75N92020D00001, HHSN268201500003I, N01-HC-95159,75N92020D00005,N01-HC-95160,75N92020D00002,N01-HC-95161,75N92020D00003, N01-HC-95162,75N92020D00006,N01-HC-95163,75N92020D00004,N01-HC-95164, 75N92020D00007,N01-HC-95165, N01-HC-95166, N01-HC-95167, N01-HC-95168 and N01-HC-95169 and grant R01 HL127659 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, and by grants UL1-TR-000040, UL1-TR-001079, and UL1-TR-001420 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RN designed the study, performed data analysis, and drafted the work. SH designed the study, obtained funding, and acquired data. RM obtained funding. NB, MH, RS, AW, and SR acquired data. RN, AEF, RM, SR, NB, AF, MH, WL, RS, AW, and SH substantially revised the work and approved the submitted version and agreed to be accountable for the integrity and accuracy of all aspects of the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robin M. Nance.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

SR reports consulting fees from ApniMed Inc, Eli Lily Inc, and Jazz Pharma- unrelated to this work and funding from NIH. RN, AEF, RM, NB, AF, MH, WL, RS, AW, and SH declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All methods in this study were carried out in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration and other relevant ethical guidelines and regulations.

Written informed consent was obtained from all MESA participants. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at each field center and the data coordinating center. Each Institutional Review Board is certified by the U.S. Office of Human Research Protections:

Wake Forest University (IRB number IRB00008492 under Federal-wide Assurance- FWA00001435),

Columbia University (IRB number IRB00002973 under Federal-wide Assurance- FWA00002636),

Johns Hopkins University (IRB number 00001656 under Federal-wide Assurance- FWA00005752),

University of Minnesota (IRB number IRB00000438 under Federal-wide Assurance- FWA00000312),

Northwestern University (IRB number IRB00005003 under Federal-wide Assurance- FWA00001549),

University of California Los Angeles (IRB number 00000172 under Federal-wide Assurance- FWA00004642),

University of Washington (IRB number STUDY00009029 under Federal-wide Assurance- FWA00006878).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nance, R.M., Fohner, A.E., McClelland, R.L. et al. The association of upper airway anatomy with cognitive test performance: the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. BMC Neurol 23, 394 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-023-03443-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-023-03443-9

Keywords