Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Short-term test-retest-reliability of conditioned pain modulation using the cold-heat-pain method in healthy subjects and its correlation to parameters of standardized quantitative sensory testing

Abstract

Background

Conditioned Pain Modulation (CPM) is often used to assess human descending pain inhibition. Nine different studies on the test-retest-reliability of different CPM paradigms have been published, but none of them has investigated the commonly used heat-cold-pain method. The results vary widely and therefore, reliability measures cannot be extrapolated from one CPM paradigm to another. Aim of the present study was to analyse the test-retest-reliability of the common heat-cold-pain method and its correlation to pain thresholds.

Methods

We tested the short-term test-retest-reliability within 40 ± 19.9 h using a cold-water immersion (10 °C, left hand) as conditioning stimulus (CS) and heat pain (43-49 °C, pain intensity 60 ± 5 on the 101-point numeric rating scale, right forearm) as test stimulus (TS) in 25 healthy right-handed subjects (12females, 31.6 ± 14.1 years). The TS was applied 30s before (TSbefore), during (TSduring) and after (TSafter) the 60s CS. The difference between the pain ratings for TSbefore and TSduring represents the early CPM-effect, between TSbefore and TSafter the late CPM-effect. Quantitative sensory testing (QST, DFNS protocol) was performed on both sessions before the CPM assessment. Statistics: paired t-tests, Intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), standard error of measurement (SEM), smallest real difference (SRD), Pearson’s correlation, Bland-Altman analysis, significance level p < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, when necessary.

Results

Pain ratings during CPM correlated significantly (ICC: 0.411…0.962) between both days, though ratings for TSafter were lower on day 2 (p < 0.005). The early (day 1: 16.7 ± 11.7; day 2: 19.5 ± 11.9; ICC: 0.618, SRD: 20.2) and late (day 1: 1.7 ± 9.2; day 2: 7.6 ± 11.5; ICC: 0.178, SRD: 27.0) CPM effect did not differ significantly between both days. Both early and late CPM-effects did not correlate with the pain thresholds.

Conclusions

The short-term test-retest-reliability of the early CPM-effect using the heat-cold-pain method in healthy subjects achieved satisfying results in terms of the ICC. The SRD of the early CPM effect showed that an individual change of > 20 NRS can be attributed to a real change rather than chance. The late CPM-effect was weaker and not reliable.

Peer Review reports

Background

Human pain modulation is of growing interest for pain research. The balance between inhibitory and facilitatory pain systems is suggested to be disrupted in several pain syndromes, e.g. fibromyalgia, irritable bowel syndrome or osteoarthritis [13]. Furthermore, ineffective endogenous analgesia seems to put patients at risk for developing chronic postoperative pain [4, 5].

Experimentally, the descending inhibitory pathways can be assessed using two noxious stimuli [6]. Plenty of paradigms assessing the conditioned pain modulation (CPM) have been described, using thermal, mechanical, electrical or pressure stimuli as test stimulus (TS) and/ or conditioning stimulus (CS) in different combinations. The so-called CPM-effect is typically calculated as the difference between pain ratings of the TS before and the TS during or - to analyse its persistence - after application of the CS [7, 8]. Hitherto, there is no consensus whether a certain CPM protocol is preferable over the others [9].

So far, only few studies have analysed the test-retest-reliability of different CPM paradigms for periods between 15 min and 10 months with sample sizes between 12 and 190 subjects, most of them in healthy subjects, using different TS (heat pain, electrical stimulation, pressure or ischemia) and CS (hot or cold-water baths, occlusion cuff) [1018]. However, one study focused on intra-individual variances of the CPM-effect elicited by different CS in 12 healthy men [17], while another focused on the influence of ongoing pain on the CPM-effect [18], both of them not examining genuine test-retest-reliability. Three further studies analysed additionally gender-specific test-retest-reliability [13, 16, 18]. To sum up, the results regarding the ICC vary widely between the studies and seem to depend on the used CS and TS and the time interval [7, 16, 17]. For most paradigms and parameters ICC analysis revealed good to excellent test-retest reliability in healthy subjects with some exceptions. Especially for the CPM-effect elicited by cuff occlusion as CS and pressure pain as TS, the ICC revealed poor reliability (ICC −0.4) over a period of 3 days [10], whereas retest with the same CPM paradigm within less than 60 min showed good to excellent ICC [10, 15]. Two studies examining the CPM-effect using electrical stimulation as TS found good test-retest reliability over 1–4 weeks based on both the nociceptive flexor reflex (NFR) response and subjective pain ratings [11, 12], but it was poor when calculated based on the electrical pain detection threshold [12], though the CPM effect based on subjective pain ratings was more reliable than based on the NFR during innocuous stimulation as control condition [11]. The authors concluded that the subjective pain ratings and objective electrophysiological measures reflect different components of the CPM [11, 12]. In contrast, examining the CPM effect in patients with chronic pain over an interval of about 1 week with painful cold stimulus as CS and pressure pain as TS, the test-retest reliability seems to be poor in males, whereas a subanalysis in female patients showed better test-retest reliability according to the ICC [13, 14, 18]. Therefore, extrapolation of reliability measures from one CPM paradigm to another and between different study populations, i.e. healthy subjects vs. patients with chronic pain, seems to be inappropriate. Only two of the above studies reported clinically relevant reliability measures like standard error of measurement and standard real difference [12, 18], though recently a review on studies addressing the test-retest reliability of sensory testing demanded for more detailed statistical evaluations of test-retest data, including also assessment of agreement of the datasets, and more transparent data presentation [19].

To prove the value of a testing paradigm for clinical applicability, an analysis of the test-retest reliability in healthy subjects is an essential prerequisite, as the confounding factors in healthy subjects are less pronounced than in patients with an underlying disease. To our knowledge, a detailed test-retest-reliability analysis in healthy subjects for the commonly used method with tonic heat as TS and tonic cold as CS [7, 20] is still lacking, although this protocol seems to provide clinically relevant information and is easily applicable, e.g. without recording EMG activity. This paradigm was recently applied in patients with painful diabetic neuropathy and was able to identify patients with insufficient endogenous analgesia who were responders to duloxetine, which is supposed to enhance the function of the descending inhibitory pathways by reuptake inhibition of serotonin and noradrenaline [21]. To evaluate the methodological stability of this CPM paradigm for the clinical practice, we analysed its short-term test-retest-reliability (24–72 h) in healthy subjects (primary objective). We analysed the difference between the pain intensity of the TS before and (i) during the simultaneous application of the CS (“early CPM-effect”) as well as (ii) shortly after the application of the CS (“late CPM-effect”).

Somatosensory function can also be examined by quantitative sensory testing (QST). The QST-protocol of the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS) is reliable and well validated [2224]. It contains, among others, the determination of thermal and mechanical pain thresholds as well as suprathreshold testing with mechanical pinprick stimuli for measurement of the mechanical pain sensitivity (MPS) and the wind-up ratio (WUR) [22]. Increased pain sensitivity to pinpricks and enhanced wind-up represent two mechanisms of central sensitization, implying a preponderance of the facilitatory pathways [25, 26]. Hence, these QST parameters might also indirectly reflect the state of the descending inhibitory pathways. Therefore, we also analysed their correlation to the magnitude of the CPM-effect (secondary objective).

Methods

Subjects

The study protocol was in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local ethics committee of the Faculty of Medicine, Ruhr-University Bochum, Germany (Reg. Nr. 4321–12, NCT01618604).

Volunteers were recruited from June 2012 to December 2012 among students, their relatives or employees of the University Hospital Bergmannsheil in Bochum, Germany. Before starting the assessment, the study was described in its entirety to the subjects, who all gave written informed consent. All subjects received a reimbursement of 70 € for completion of the study. Inclusion criteria were age above 18 years, right-handedness (assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [27]), absence of chronic pain, no drug intake, especially no use of analgesic drugs in the last 14 days. Each subject was asked to answer a screening tool for healthy subjects, which has previously been established within the IMI-Europain consortium (www.imi.europa.eu). It comprises the following exclusion criteria: age under 18 years, missing informed consent, insufficient German language skills, current or recent pain, recent intake of analgesic or other drugs (except contraceptives), consume of alcohol or energy drinks, history of neurological, dermatological, chronic internal or psychiatric diseases, abnormal neurological examination, recent sleep restriction or unusual physical exercises, and pregnancy. Relevant depression and anxiety symptoms were additionally excluded by applying the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) prior to study begin on the first session [28, 29]. Other study-specific exclusion criteria were abnormal results in the baseline quantitative sensory testing (QST) with values outside the 95 % confidence interval for healthy subjects according to the DFNS reference database [24] in order to exclude subjects with incidental or subclinical neuropathy.

One female was excluded because of an abnormal side-to-side difference for thermal detection thresholds, indicating a unilateral neuropathy. None of the subjects was excluded due to one of the above-mentioned criteria concerning the pain ratings during the CPM procedure. Thus, a final sample size of twenty-five subjects (mean age: 31.6 ± 14.3 (21…69) years; 12 females, n = 3 > 40 years; 13 males, n = 1 > 40 years) was used for further statistical analyses.

Study design

Subjects attended two sessions 24 to 72 h apart. Both sessions were conducted in exactly the same way and were performed in the afternoon (between 4 pm and 7 pm) by the same female examiner (J.G.) in the certified QST laboratory in the University Hospital Bergmannsheil GmbH in Bochum, Germany. On day 1, each subject answered the German Version of the Pain Sensitivity Questionnaire (PSQ) [30] to assess self-reported pain sensitivity in relation to daily life situations for correlation with the results of the experimental setting (secondary outcome).

The test procedure is illustrated in Fig. 1. It strictly adhered to a standardized script with standardized instructions, which were read out to the subjects. The assessment began with the determination of the individual test stimulus temperature (TSinitial), followed by QST according to the protocol of the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS) on the right and left dorsum of the hand, lasting about 60 min. After that, we applied the established CPM paradigm as previously described by Granot et al. [7] with the initially defined test stimulus (TS) and a fixed conditioning stimulus (CS) (details see below). Adverse events were protocolled during the testing procedure until 30 min after the end of the testing procedures.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study design

The protocols for QST and CPM assessment are adapted from Rolke et al. [22] and Granot et al. [7], respectively. DFNS, German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain; NRS, Numerical Rating Scale; QST, quantitative sensory testing; CPM, conditioned pain modulation.

Conditioned pain modulation assessment

Test stimulus calibration

The TSinitial was redefined every day for each subject as the heat stimulus temperature rated with 60 ± 5 on the NRS (0–100) [31].

Heat stimuli were set with a thermal sensory testing device (TSA 2001-II, MEDOC, Israel, CoVAS software, version 3.20) using a thermode with a contact area of 30x30mm and a stimulus ramp of 4 °C/s. First, subjects rated the pain intensity on the NRS (0–100) during a standard heat battery consisting of three stimuli (45, 46 and 47 °C) with a duration of 7 s and an inter-stimulus interval of 35 s to determine the individual TS temperature. The heat stimuli were applied on the right volar forearm and the thermode was moved 30 mm proximally after the first, and 30 mm distally from the middle of the forearm after the second stimulus to prevent thermal sensitization [32]. In case the standard temperatures (45-47 °C) were rated as too painful or not painful enough, two further stimuli of lower or higher temperature, respectively, were used (43 and 44 °C or 48 and 49 °C).

Conditioning stimulus

The CS was delivered by immersion of the left hand up to the wrist in a cold-water bath kept at 10 °C for 60s. The temperature was measured by a calibrated quicksilver thermometer (0-100 °C) with an accuracy of ±1 °C. The CS was not individually calibrated, in accordance with the established protocol by Granot et al. [7] and other studies confirming sufficient induction of a CPM-effect with this type and intensity of the CS [10, 33].

CPM procedure

The individually determined TS temperature, corresponding to a pain intensity of 60 ± 5 on the NRS (0–100) was applied for 30 s three times during the CPM assessment as TSbefore, TSduring and TSafter according to the protocol by Granot et al. [7]. Each time, the subjects were asked to rate the heat pain intensity after 10, 20, and 30 s on the NRS (0–100) (see Fig. 1). Five minutes after the first TS (TSbefore), subjects were asked to put their hand into the cold water with spread fingers and without touching the bottom or the walls of the container. Subjects rated the cold pain intensity after 30 and 60 s (NRS, 0–100). After 30 s immersion of the hand in the cold-water bath, the TS was applied simultaneously (TSduring) for 30 s. Subjects rated the pain intensity again with focus on the TS after 10, 20 and 30 s. Pain ratings for the TS after 30s and the CS after 60s were made separately at the same time, concentrating on the intensity of heat and cold pain, respectively. Finally, 5 min after termination of the CS, the TS was applied again for 30 s (TSafter) and its intensity was rated after 10, 20 and 30 s. 30 min after the TSafter, the CS was applied again solitarily for 60 s and its intensity was rated after 30 and 60 s (NRS, 0–100) to analyse distraction effects during simultaneous application of TS and CS.

Calculation of the early CPM-effect

The extent of the endogeneous analgesia was calculated as the difference between the mean of the three pain ratings for the TSbefore (after 10, 20 and 30s) and the mean of the three pain ratings for the TSduring (after 10, 20 and 30s), in accordance to previous studies [4, 7] and was defined as early CPM-effect [4, 7].

$$ Early\ CPM- effect = Mean\ of\ three\ pain\ ratings\ T{S}_{before}\hbox{--}\ Mean\ of\ three\ pain\ ratings\ T{S}_{during}. $$

Calculation of the late CPM-effect

In order to evaluate the 5-min-persistence of the CPM-effect (late CPM-effect), we furthermore calculated the difference between the mean of the three pain ratings for the TSbefore and the mean of the three pain ratings for the TSafter.

$$ Late\ CPM- effect = Mean\ of\ three\ pain\ ratings\ T{S}_{before}\hbox{--}\ Mean\ of\ three\ pain\ ratings\ T{S}_{after}. $$

Quantitative sensory testing

In accordance to the protocol of the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS), the QST assessment included seven tests measuring 13 parameters [22] and was performed on the left and right dorsum of the hand. Before starting the QST assessment, all subjects became familiarized with the stimuli in an area other than the area to be tested. This standardized test battery consisted of the cold detection threshold (CDT), warm detection threshold (WDT), thermal sensory limen (TSL), paradoxical heat sensations (PHS), cold pain threshold (CPT), heat pain threshold (HPT), mechanical detection threshold (MDT), mechanical pain threshold (MPT), mechanical pain sensitivity (MPS), dynamic mechanical allodynia (DMA), wind-up ratio (WUR), vibration detection threshold (VDT) and pressure pain threshold (PPT). For the warm and cold detection the same TSA 2001-II (MEDOC, Israel, Win TSA software, version 5.29) thermal sensory testing device was used as for the CPM assessment (32 °C baseline temperature, stimulus ramp of 1 °C/s, cut-off values 0 °C - 50 °C). The mechanical detection threshold (MDT) was measured using modified von Frey filaments (Optihair2-Set, Marstock Nervtest, Schriesheim, Germany) between 0,25 and 512mN. Modified Pin Pricks (MRC Systems, Heidelberg, Germany) from 8 to 512mN were used for the MPT, WUR and MPS. Light tactile stimuli (cotton wool, brush and Q-tip) were used to assess DMA. The VDT was determined with a Rydel-Seiffer tuning fork (64 Hz, 8/8 scale) that was placed over the ulnar styloid process. The PPT was measured by a pressure algometer placed over the thenar muscle (FDN200, Wagner Instruments, Greenwich, USA, probe area: 1 cm2, stimulus ramp 0.5 kg/s, 2-20 kg/cm2).

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using SPSS, Version 20 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). Normal distribution of variables was assured using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Within group differences (e.g., pain ratings day 1 vs. day 2) were analysed using paired t-tests. For analysis of the relative test-retest reliability between the two CPM-assessments, i.e., the degree to which the subjects’ measurements or scores maintained their position relative to others, we used indices of agreement, the Pearson’s product–moment correlation, followed by calculation of the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC). To control for bias between measurement on both days, ICC analyses were conducted using a two-way mixed effects model with terms of absolute agreement. The standard error of measurement and its 95 % confidence interval, the smallest real difference (SRD) were calculated as absolute measure for the reliability of both the early and late CPM effect (standard error of measurement = intra-individual standard deviation * √(1-ICC), SRD = 1.96 * standard error of measurement * √2\( \Big) \) [34]. Both parameters were calculated to reflect the sensitivity of change of the early and late CPM effect. Measuring the magnitude of the CPM effects, it is not only important to calculate the ICC, but also to be able to evaluate changes due to an intervention e.g. a pharmacological treatment. The standard error of measurement indicates the expected error between two measurements conducted under the same circumstances in the same subject over a defined period of time, and should not be confused with the standard error of mean (also abbreviated as SEM). The lower the standard error of measurement, the better the test-retest-reliability [34, 35]. The SRD, as the 95 % confidence interval of the SEM, indicates the change in value that cannot be interpreted as random scatter between two measurements in an individual, but has to be assigned to change of circumstances, i.e. a treatment or intervention effect. Bland-Altman-plots were conducted as a graphical interpretation [36], displaying the relationship between the mean CPM-effect on day 1 and day 2 on the x-axis, and the difference between CPM effects of day 2 and day 1 for each subject on the y-axis, separately for both early and late CPM-effect. An important part of an analysis of reliability is the assessment not only of mean differences, but also of the variance of these differences, as reliability is more important for single subjects than for groups of subjects [37]. In Bland-Altman-plots, both individual effects and effects estimated on the basis of the study group can be seen: the mean difference (i.e. bias) between day 2 - day 1 is marked as a bold line and the estimated 95 % limits of agreement (LoA) as thin lines, and their 95 % confidence intervals as dashed lines. The 95 % LoA represent the range of limits in which 95 % of the data are expected to lie, based on the study population [37], their confidence intervals indicate the level of uncertainty due to the variance in the dataset and the limited number of subjects in the study population. The lower the limits, the closer the values between both measurements and the better the reliability.

As recommended by Biurrun Manresa et al. [12], the clinical relevance of reliability results can become evident by calculating the estimated samples sizes. Therefore we calculated sample sizes for the early and late CPM effect considering crossover (i.e. intragroup reliability analysis, sample size = number of subjects each receiving different assessments) and parallel (i.e. intergroup reliability analysis, sample size = number of subjects for each group) study designs, following the guidelines described by Julious et al. [38]. The calculation is based on the question how many subjects are needed to treat (e.g. drugs, intervention) to elevate a former “non responder” (mean CPM effect = 0) to a “normal responder” (in the case of the present study population: mean early CPM effect = 18 NRS points, and mean late CPM effect = 4 NRS points, see results). For this purpose, we built four subgroups analysing 25, 50, 75 and finally 100 % success of treatment.

Questionnaire data from the PSQ was correlated with the early and late CPM-effect using the Pearson correlation to compare self-reported information on pain sensitivity in daily-life situations and the function of the descending pathways during the experimental setting. Additionally, the HADS scores were correlated with the early and late CPM-effect using the Pearson correlation to assess any association with the magnitude of depression or anxiety scores (within the normal range).

For all analyses comprising QST data, data of the right hand was used. QST raw data was logarithmically transformed, except for PHS, CPT, HPT and PPT, as previously described [24]. All parameters (except PHS and DMA) were z-transformed to compare the data independently from age, gender and region, based on the existing DFNS reference data base [24] using the QST data analysis program eQuiSTA (Casquar GmbH, Bochum, Germany). Test-retest reliability analyses of the QST raw data were performed using the ICC.

Pearson’s product moment correlation was used to assess the association between the QST raw values and the early and the late CPM-effect, respectively. Furthermore, we conducted a median-split analysis, separating subgroups according to the magnitude of the early and late CPM-effect (early CPM-effect: < 15 and ≥ 15; late CPM: effect < 3 and ≥ 3, respectively) and comparing raw data of each QST-parameter (after log-transformation) between these subgroups using the Mann–Whitney-U test in order to detect discrete differences between both subgroups, which do not appear in the correlation analysis considering the whole group.

A p-value of 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant, and Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were applied for each test group.

Results

Subjects’ characteristics and baseline measures

Measurements were repeated within 40 ± 19.9 h. In all subjects a TSinitial with a pain rating of 60 ± 5 on the NRS (0–100) could be determined at both days with a temperature between 45 and 49 °C. 6 subjects had a difference of 1 °C in their TSinitial between both days. All subjects could tolerate the 10 °C cold-water bath for 60s. Mild erythema was detected in all subjects in the application area of the thermode, disappearing within 30 min after testing. Two subjects complained about pain up to the shoulder and one about nausea during the cold-water immersion.

CPM magnitude on day 1 and day 2

On each day, all but one subject (96 %) had an early CPM-effect >0, indicating that the endogenous pain inhibition during simultaneous cold-water immersion was induced in nearly all subjects. The subject without positive early CPM-effect on both days was not the same. Regarding the whole study group on both days, the pain rating for the TS before application of the CS (TSbefore) significantly decreased during the simultaneous CS application (TSduring, p < 0.01), indicating also a statistically relevant early CPM-effect (see Fig. 2a). The size of the early CPM-effect was 28 on day 1 and 33 % on day 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Graphic illustration of the CPM effect’s calculation. a Whisker plots of the mean of three pain ratings for TSbefore and TSduring, resulting in the early CPM-effect. b Whisker plots of the mean of three pain ratings for TSbefore and TSafter, resulting in the late CPM-effect. The bottom and the top of the boxes represent the first and third quartiles, the band inside is the median. The ends of the whiskers illustrate the maximum and minimum. TS were applied before, during and after the conditioning stimulus. CPM, conditioned pain modulation; NRS, numeric rating scale; TS, test stimulus

In contrast, a late CPM-effect > 0 was seen only in 14 subjects on day 1 (56 %) and 16 subjects on day 2 (64 %), indicating that endogenous pain inhibition lasted longer than 5 min after termination of the CS only in part of the subjects. 9 subjects (36 %) showed a late CPM-effect on both days, 4 subjects (16 %) presented no late CPM-effect on both days. The group analysis revealed a significant decrease of the pain ratings after CS application (TSbefore vs. TSafter) only on day 2 (p < 0.01, day 1: p = 0.376; see Fig. 2b). The size of the late CPM-effect was 3 on day 1 and 15 % on day 2.

Significant correlations between the magnitude of the pain ratings for the cold water immersion (CS) and the resulting CPM effect could not be observed.

Test-retest-reliability of conditioned pain modulation

After Bonferroni correction for multiple testing there were no significant differences between both days regarding the reported pain intensities (NRS, 0–100) for TSinitial, TSbefore, TSduring, CS30s and CS60s, the early CPM-effect or the late CPM-effect (Table 2). Only the pain ratings for TSafter were significantly lower on day 2.

Each of the individual parameters of the CPM protocol demonstrated a significant close to moderate correlation between both days based on the ICC (Table 1). Concerning the composite parameters, the correlation of the early CPM-effect between both days was good; however, there was no significant correlation of the late CPM-effect between both days.

Table 1 Parameters of the Conditioned Pain Modulation (CPM) assessment

For the early CPM-effect the standard error of measurement (SEM) was 7.3 on the 0–100 NRS scale, representing 40 % of the mean CPM-effect (Table 2). For the late CPM-effect, the SEM was 211 % as big as the mean (Table 2). The smallest real difference (SRD) indicated that a change larger than 20.2 NRS points for the early CPM effect, and 27.0 NRS points for the late CPM effect in an individual case has to be assigned to a real change and not random scatter between measurements (Table 2).

Table 2 Test-retest-reliability analyses for the mean early and late CPM effect (day 1 vs. day 2), n = 25

Given the fact, that a CPM “non-responder” might have a CPM effect of 0, and a “normal responder” might have a mean CPM effect with 18 NRS points for the early CPM effect, and 4 NRS points for the late CPM effect (Table 2), we calculated hypothetical sample sizes for crossover and parallel designs in terms of successful treatment (Table 3). In case of 50 % treatment success after an intervention, 18 subjects are needed to confirm this hypothesis in the crossover design, and 68 subjects in the parallel design for the early CPM effect. For the late CPM effect the number of subjects needed to confirm such a hypothesis is beyond the clinical relevance.

Table 3 Hypothetical sample size calculations for crossover and parallel designs in terms of successful treatment

The Bland-Altman Plot for the early CPM effect (Fig. 3a) showed a range for the 95 limits of agreement (LoA) between −17.4 (95 CI: −21.5…-13.4) NRS points (0–100) and 23.0 (95 % CI: 19.0…27.1) NRS points. The late CPM effect (Fig. 3b) showed a range for the 95 LoA between −20.2 (95 CI: −25.4… -15.0) NRS points and 32.1 (95 % CI: 26.9… 37.3) NRS points (Table 2). Regarding the absolute range, the limits for the late CPM effect were approximately 30 % wider than for the early CPM effect. Figure 3a shows all values evenly distributed around the mean difference (bold line = bias), 11/25 subjects are below the mean difference, 14/25 subjects above, indicating no systematic deviation between the measurements, that could be assigned, for example, to learning effects (correlation r = 0.019, p = 0.928). The mean early CPM effect between both days ranges between 0–50 NRS points, the mean difference between −17 and + 18 NRS points. The range for the mean late CPM effect between both days is between −8 and 25 NRS points, the mean difference between −19 and +33 NRS points.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Bland-Altman plot for the CPM-effect on day 1 and the difference between the CPM-effect on day 2 and day 1. a early CPM-effect (r =0.019, p = 0.928), b late CPM-effect (r = 0.215, p = 0.302). The bold line is the mean difference of the CPM-effect of both days, the dashed lines represent the 95 % limits of agreement. CPM, Conditioned Pain Modulation; NRS, numeric rating scale

The early and late CPM-effect did not correlate significantly neither on day 1 (r = 0.330, p = 0.107) nor on day 2 (r = 0.375, p = 0.065).

Quantitative sensory testing

Test-retest reliability of quantitative sensory testing

After z-transformation, all subjects showed QST parameters within the normal range between −1.96 and 1.96, as expected. One male subject (63 years) reported 1 (of 3) paradoxical heat sensations on day 1. None of the subjects had dynamic mechanical allodynia.

In the whole study group, all QST parameters correlated significantly between day 1 and day 2 (ICC = 0.450…0.916) except for the cold detection threshold (CDT, ICC = 0.265).

Correlation of quantitative sensory testing and CPM-effects

There were no significant correlations between the magnitude of the early and late CPM-effect and all QST parameters, which were present at both days. After Bonferroni correction, the correlation between the heat pain threshold (HPT) and the late CPM-effect on day 2 was not significant (r = − 0.401, p = 0.047, Table 4). Likewise, median split analyses regarding the early and late CPM-effect revealed no significant differences between all QST parameters, which occurred on both days, especially regarding the thermal (CPT: p = 0.609…0.936, HPT: p = 0.344…0.936) and mechanical pain thresholds (MPT: p = 0.244…0.979, PPT: p = 0.205…0.467) and parameters of the stimulus–response-function (MPS: p = 0.046…0.809, WUR, p = 0.470…0.979).

Table 4 Correlational analyses between CPM effects and QST parameters at day 1 and day 2

Questionnaires

The scores for the HADS were within the range for healthy subjects (subscore for anxiety: 3.0 ± 2.2 (0…5); subscore for depression: 1.4 ± 1.7 (0…5)). The mean PSQ scores (3.4 ± 1.4 (0.6…6.1) were similar to the data for healthy subjects originally reported by the authors (3.6 ± 1.2 [30]).

Regarding the whole study group, no significant correlations were found between PSQ, HADS overall score and subscores for anxiety and depression and the early or late CPM-effect.

Discussion

To summarize, we induced endogenous pain inhibition with CPM-effect >0 using cold-water immersion as CS in almost all healthy subjects on both days. However, in >1/3 of them it lasted for less than 5 min after the CS termination. The absolute magnitude of the early CPM-effect (during simultaneous CS application) was similar on both days and was consistent with most of the previous studies [20], though in contrast to some others, which reported lower [39, 40] or higher CPM-effects [17, 41, 42]. For the early CPM-effect, the test-retest-reliability within the period of 24-72 h assessed in healthy subjects showed a SEM of about 40 % of the mean and a SRD of 20 (on the NRS 0–100). In contrast, the reliability of the late CPM-effect, assessed 5 min after CS termination, was rather insufficient, represented in a SRD nearly six times bigger than the mean effect, making the detection of a real change between two assessments within a subject nearly impossible and estimated sample sizes for experimental studies rather unrealistic. Neither thermal nor mechanical pain thresholds, nor QST parameters comprising suprathreshold stimuli correlated with either CPM-effect.

Differences between the CPM paradigms regarding the test-re-test-reliability

So far, nine studies have been published analysing the test-retest reliability of different CPM paradigms, than the one we used [1018]. Three of them analysed the reliability in chronic pain patients or in an experimental pain model for acute musculoskeletal pain [13, 14, 18]. The remaining six studies analysed the test-retest-reliability in healthy subjects using the nociceptive withdrawal reflex (NWR), electric, pressure or heat pain as TS and cold- or hot-water baths and ischemia as CS. Three studies reported similar ICC as in our study ranging between 0.54 and 0.69 [10, 11, 15]. However, two studies reported insufficient reliability based on ICC: Biurrun et al. used NWR thresholds, electrical pain thresholds and suprathreshold electrical pain as TS and cold-water bath as CS with ICC of 0.09 to 0.44 within 1–3 weeks for suprathreshold electrical pain [12]. Another study in healthy females used heat both as TS and CS and achieved ICC = 0.39 for retests over a period of 7–10 months [16]. In contrast, our study concentrated on the short-term test-retest-reliability, which might explain the better ICC, pointing to a more stable CPM-effect over shorter periods of time. Choosing an appropriate time period for reassessment is an important aspect. While re-assessment over very short time periods of 60 min or less [Cathcard2009; Lewis2012] might be more reliable, very short time periods might be insufficient to expect a real treatment effect when examining e.g. changes in the CPM-effect due to an intervention. On the other hand longer time periods between reassessments implicate changed external conditions which might influence the results, especially when examining patients, also in relatively short term of about 1 week [13, 14] which is pronounced in male patients [13]. There is also some evidence, that different CPM paradigms engage different spinal or supraspinal inhibitory mechanisms [4345]; e.g., the magnitude and stability of CPM-effects with the use of subjectively reported pain intensities was shown to be stronger than objective measures like the NWR [44, 45].

Furthermore, most studies reported only ICC values as measure for test-retest-reliability [10, 11, 1316]. One study showed higher ICC values for the CPM-effect based on subjective pain ratings compared to electrophysiological responses during the NWR (ICC 0.44 vs 0.26, [12]), while another reported similar ICC values over 28 days for both TS (ICC 0.54 vs 0.61, [11]). It has been previously discussed that cognitive influences may represent a stable confounder between test and retest session, thus explaining the slightly better reliability in our study in comparison to a study examining the CPM elicited by the same CS based on electrophysiological measures [12]. Only a few studies conveyed ICC analysis also of the single parameters during CPM assessment, i.e. of CS as well as TS before, during and after CS application). In terms of the ICC, the pain rating of the CS in our study indicated excellent reliability with higher values (ICC 0.77-0.95) than previously reported when using the same CS in patients with chronic pain (ICC 0.61-0.67) [13] and similar ICC as when heat was used as CS (ICC 0.79) [16]. Though tonic heat has been previously suggested to be more constant and less confounded by changes in cardiovascular activity in healthy subjects compared with cold water stimuli [11, 46], our results suggest that both conditioning stimuli are at least comparable regarding their retest-reliability. The current evidence suggests that both tonic cold and heat stimuli as CS seem to be superior to cuff occlusion as CS, as the latter was able to induce a reliable CPM effect only up to 60 min, but not over a period of 3 days, [10, 15].

Only two reported SEM and SRD values [12, 18], which allow much more realistic estimation of the clinical relevance in terms of evaluating the outcome after intervention based on the CPM-effect. While Biurrun et al. [12] studied a completely different CPM-paradigm with electrical stimulation as TS, Valencia et al. assessed similar protocol as ours with repeatedly applied heat pulses as TS and cold pain as CS and reported a minimal detectable change of about 17 in the healthy cohort for re-test measurement within two minutes independently on the fact whether additionally the healthy subjects experienced exercise induced muscle pain or not [18].

For the first time, we report a smallest real difference for the commonly used protocol within a re-test period of 24-72 h, an interval which might be more relevant in term of evaluation a therapeutic interventions based on the CPM-effect. Our results depict that, using the presented CPM-protocol with respect to the early CPM-effect, a smallest real difference more than 20 points on the 0–100 NRS between two measurements in a healthy individual is relevant and can be assigned to change of circumstances, and is not a random scatter. In accordance to our findings, in a subgroup of patients with polyneuropathy, who did not have any pain relief after duloxetin treatment, presented with a more efficient CPM already prior to treatment and the CPM-effect changed on average about 10 on a 101-point NRS after treatment [21]. In contrast, in the subgroup of patients with sufficient pain relief after treatment with duloxetin, the change in CPM-effect was on average 15. This represents a value below the calculated SRD in our study and indicates that in pathological states with impaired CPM even smaller changes than in healthy states, might be clinical relevant. On the other hand, it implicates that further studies examining the test-retest-reliability in patients with chronic pain and CPM impairment without intervention between the measurements are needed to strengthen this hypothesis.

Only one study evaluated the reliability of a CPM paradigm by calculating sample sizes for potential experiments, thus using a clinically more relevant parameter than the pure calculation of the ICC [12]. Under the experimental conditions described here, the sample sizes and consequently the reliability of the early CPM-effect are certainly acceptable and realistic for experimental or clinical use, especially for a crossover design (Table 3). For example, in case of an intervention, which is intended to normalize the CPM-effect in 50 % of the treated patients, who were previously incapable to activate their CPM, one would need a study sample of at least 18 subjects for crossover and 68 subjects for parallel design to detect a significant change after the intervention. Such estimations for calculating hypothetical sample sizes should be demanded also for other studies on re-test-reliability, as they provide a much more realistic measure for the clinical relevance of the data than statistical measures like the ICC.

Persistence of the CPM effect after termination of the conditioning stimulus

In our study, the late CPM-effect, i.e. endogenous inhibition lasting for at least 5 min after CS termination, was smaller than the early CPM-effect, in line with previous studies [8, 41, 47, 48]. It also varied largely between both days, though statistically not significant after Bonferroni correction, resulting in its insufficient test-retest-reliability and unacceptably high sample sizes in comparison to those for the early CPM-effect to detect the same effect. Even though there were no correlations between the magnitude of the CS rating and the resulting CPM-effect, one might argue that the early CPM-effect is simply the result of distraction by the painful cold-water bath. Though, combined distraction and CPM induced greater pain reduction than either alone [49]. Moreover, different extents of cortical activation in frontal and somatosensory areas were found by distraction and by CPM [50]. Our findings point to an influence of distraction on CPM, but nevertheless support the existence of a “real” CPM-effect, which however seems to have a higher interindividual variability. One study reported no late CPM-effect at all [51], whereas another found greater and longer lasting effects of pain inhibition [52], both assessing the CPM-effect as change in the TS intensity (pain threshold) and not of the pain intensity as in our protocol. These inconsistent findings indicate that the duration of the CPM-effect after CS termination might differ depending on the applied stimuli, but also depending on the chosen read-out, i.e. change in pain intensity of a predefined stimulus or change of pain threshold.

Correlations between CPM and detection/ pain thresholds as well as suprathreshold stimulation

Another objective of the present study was to assess possible correlations between the CPM-effects and parameters of a standardized QST-protocol. Theoretically, subjects can be positioned in a continuum between pro- and anti-nociception, e.g., a “pro-nociceptive subject” would have a low CPM-effect and show low pain thresholds and high pain ratings for suprathreshold stimuli in his sensory profile [9]. Surprisingly, we found no correlations between QST parameters and the early or late CPM-effect. Previous studies on CPM using pain thresholds as TS showed sufficient CPM-effects resulting in pain threshold increase [2, 53, 54]. However, whilst others analysed pain thresholds as TS, i.e. read-out for the CPM-effect, we analysed for the first time correlations between the sensory profile and the CPM-magnitude. Despite applying the same stimuli, our CPM paradigm uses suprathreshold stimuli, whereas during the QST according to the DFNS-protocol mainly subthreshold stimuli are applied, i.e. until the first perception of pain. These thermal and mechanical pain thresholds did not correlate with the early and late CPM-effect. Suprathreshold stimulation within the DFNS-protocol is only conducted using pinpricks assessing the mechanical pain sensitivity and the wind-up ratio, but they were also not associated with the CPM-effects, probably pointing to different neuronal pathways [21, 22, 43, 55]. Given the high reliability of the QST parameters of the DFNS-protocol [23], replicated in this study, the rather large variability of the CPM magnitude seems not to influence the parameters of the sensory profile.

Influencing factors on the magnitude of the CPM-effect

Depression and anxiety have been shown to partly influence pain thresholds and suprathreshold testing [5559] and are linked to neurotransmitters deficiency including serotonin and dopamine, which are also involved in the descending nociceptive inhibitory pathways [6062]. As expected, in our sample of heathy subjects all HADS scores were within the normal range [29, 63]. Thus, analysing the relationship between these psychiatric symptoms and CPM was not diagnostically conclusive, though there was no correlation with the early or the late CPM-effect. Also the PSQ exploring the perception of potential daily life pain [30] did not correlate with CPM-effects. This was somehow unexpected, as the PSQ score was reported to correlate significantly with experimental pain ratings in healthy subjects having similar PSQ scores as our study population [30]. However, because PSQ-score correlates with QST parameters [30] and QST does not correlate with CPM-effects in the present study our results are not unexpected. For pain patients any associations to both psychometric scales should be further explored.

Limitations

In our study, we used an established protocol [7], whereupon the TS calibration was conducted with a 7 s tonic heat stimulus, while the TS during the CPM procedure lasted 30 s. Time is a critical parameter with regard to pain intensity, as either habituation or sensitisation can occur during application of long-lasting stimuli. In future studies, the same length of TS application for the calibration and during CPM-testing should be considered. Another point was that the CS was not individually calibrated. Though the used cold-water immersion was reported to be a sufficient CS [7, 17], the high variability of the perceived cold pain intensity might account for the lack of individualized CS calibration. As a psychophysical method, CPM is susceptible to emotional and cognitive factors, such as expectation [64, 65], stress [40] or distraction [40, 49], which might play a role in our study as subjects were not initially familiarized with the CPM procedure. This might explain the higher CPM-effects on day 2, when they were more accustomed to the situation. For future studies, familiarization with the experimental set-up should be obligate. Also experienced and non-experienced subjects should not be included into one study, and information about the familiarization procedures should be reported in detail.

The lack of control task is another limitation of the study, because a pain habituation after repeated application of the TS, accounting partly for the observed pain reduction, cannot be excluded, as thermal heat pain applied by a thermode significantly habituated within the first 6 stimuli [66]. Generally, one main critic against the concept of conditioned pain modulation is the fact that the CPM-effects might be just the result of peripheral habituation or distraction (see above). However, it could be demonstrated that the CPM-experimental paradigm produced significantly more pain reduction than the habituation paradigm and the paradigm involving non-noxious inhibitory control [67]. Granot et al. [7] using the very same CPM protocol as we did, have also found that only immersion of the dominant hand in 12 °C cold and 46.5 °C hot water, but not in the conditions with less painful cold or warm water, elicited a significant CPM-effect comparable to that in our study.

Due to organisational reasons the time interval between both CPM-assessments ranged between 1–3 days. Thus, possible learning effects might have influenced the results of subjects with 24-h-intervals in-between CPM-assessments differently than those with longer breaks. Our aim was not to analyse gender differences, which explains the comparatively small chosen sample size. The sample variability within the group with subjects between 21 and 69 years, might have been quite high, as it is well known that age and sex can affect attentional processes. Though, to minimize such effects, we complemented our analyses for the standard error of measurement to compare not only interindividual, but also intraindividual differences. Furthermore we did not specify the menstrual cycle phase in all female subjects. On this issue, there exist divergent opinions: Rezaii et al. showed that female sex hormones modulate CPM [68], while Wilson et al. demonstrated no variations between CPM during the menstrual cycle [16]. Further reliability studies using the heat-cold-pain method for CPM are needed with a larger sample size to analyse gender and differences regarding the retest-reliability and differences during the menstrual cycle, as appropriate. Additionally, the retest-reliability for longer time-intervals such as weeks or months should be also further examined, which are more relevant for follow-ups in chronic pain patients. For principle reasons, the validity of this method in patients cannot be evaluated in healthy subjects. Accordingly, also the clinical relevance of the early and especially of the late CPM-effect needs further exploration.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we evaluated the most commonly used method for CPM with heat and cold pain as TS and CS, respectively, demonstrating a sufficient reliability for the early CPM-effect and associated parameters within 48 h, but not for the late CPM-effect. Based on the SRD and SEM as well as considering the above-mentioned limitations, sample size calculations for studies using CPM-effect, evaluated during simultaneous application of tonic heat as TS and tonic pain as CS, as a primary outcome are realistic for experimental or clinical use. Based on our hypothetical sample size calculations crossover design should be preferred rather than paralleled design, due to the high interindividual variability of endogenous analgesia.

References

  1. Chang L. Brain responses to visceral and somatic stimuli in irritable bowel syndrome: a central nervous system disorder? Gastroenterol Clin North Am. 2005;34(2):271–9. doi:10.1016/j.gtc.2005.02.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Kosek E, Ordeberg G. Lack of pressure pain modulation by heterotopic noxious conditioning stimulation in patients with painful osteoarthritis before, but not following, surgical pain relief. Pain. 2000;88(1):69–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Staud R, Robinson ME, Vierck Jr CJ, Price DD. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC) attenuate temporal summation of second pain in normal males but not in normal females or fibromyalgia patients. Pain. 2003;101(1–2):167–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Yarnitsky D, Crispel Y, Eisenberg E, Granovsky Y, Ben-Nun A, Sprecher E, et al. Prediction of chronic post-operative pain: pre-operative DNIC testing identifies patients at risk. Pain. 2008;138(1):22–8. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.10.033.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Grosen K, Vase L, Pilegaard HK, Pfeiffer-Jensen M, Drewes AM. Conditioned pain modulation and situational pain catastrophizing as preoperative predictors of pain following chest wall surgery: a prospective observational cohort study. PLoS One. 2014;9(2):e90185. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0090185.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Yarnitsky D, Granot M, Granovsky Y. Pain modulation profile and pain therapy: between pro- and antinociception. Pain. 2014;155(4):663–5. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.11.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Granot M, Weissman-Fogel I, Crispel Y, Pud D, Granovsky Y, Sprecher E, et al. Determinants of endogenous analgesia magnitude in a diffuse noxious inhibitory control (DNIC) paradigm: do conditioning stimulus painfulness, gender and personality variables matter? Pain. 2008;136(1–2):142–9. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.06.029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Fujii K, Motohashi K, Umino M. Heterotopic ischemic pain attenuates somatosensory evoked potentials induced by electrical tooth stimulation: diffuse noxious inhibitory controls in the trigeminal nerve territory. Eur J Pain. 2006;10(6):495–504. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.07.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Yarnitsky D, Bouhassira D, Drewes AM, Fillingim RB, Granot M, Hansson P, et al. Recommendations on practice of conditioned pain modulation (CPM) testing. Eur J Pain. 2014. doi:10.1002/ejp.605.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Lewis GN, Heales L, Rice DA, Rome K, McNair PJ. Reliability of the conditioned pain modulation paradigm to assess endogenous inhibitory pain pathways. Pain Res Manag. 2012;17(2):98–102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Jurth C, Rehberg B, von Dincklage F. Reliability of subjective pain ratings and nociceptive flexion reflex responses as measures of conditioned pain modulation. Pain Res Manag. 2014;19(2):93–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Biurrun Manresa JA, Fritsche R, Vuilleumier PH, Oehler C, Morch CD, Arendt-Nielsen L, et al. Is the conditioned pain modulation paradigm reliable? A test-retest assessment using the nociceptive withdrawal reflex. PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e100241. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0100241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Martel MO, Wasan AD, Edwards RR. Sex differences in the stability of conditioned pain modulation (CPM) among patients with chronic pain. Pain Med. 2013;14(11):1757–68. doi:10.1111/pme.12220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Olesen SS, van Goor H, Bouwense SA, Wilder-Smith OH, Drewes AM. Reliability of static and dynamic quantitative sensory testing in patients with painful chronic pancreatitis. Reg Anesth Pain Med. 2012;37(5):530–6. doi:10.1097/AAP.0b013e3182632c40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Cathcart S, Winefield AH, Rolan P, Lushington K. Reliability of temporal summation and diffuse noxious inhibitory control. Pain research & management : the journal of the Canadian Pain Society = journal de la societe canadienne pour le traitement de la douleur. 2009;14(6):433–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Wilson H, Carvalho B, Granot M, Landau R. Temporal stability of conditioned pain modulation in healthy women over four menstrual cycles at the follicular and luteal phases. Pain. 2013;154(12):2633–8. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Oono Y, Nie H, Matos RL, Wang K, Arendt-Nielsen L. The inter- and intra-individual variance in descending pain modulation evoked by different conditioning stimuli in healthy men. Scandinavian Journal of Pain. 2011;2(4):162–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Valencia C, Kindler LL, Fillingim RB, George SZ. Stability of conditioned pain modulation in two musculoskeletal pain models: investigating the influence of shoulder pain intensity and gender. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2013;14(1):182. doi:10.1186/1471-2474-14-182.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Werner MU, Petersen MA, Bischoff JM. Test-retest studies in quantitative sensory testing: a critical review. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2013;57(8):957–63. doi:10.1111/aas.12150.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Pud D, Granovsky Y, Yarnitsky D. The methodology of experimentally induced diffuse noxious inhibitory control (DNIC)-like effect in humans. Pain. 2009;144(1–2):16–9. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.02.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Yarnitsky D, Granot M, Nahman-Averbuch H, Khamaisi M, Granovsky Y. Conditioned pain modulation predicts duloxetine efficacy in painful diabetic neuropathy. Pain. 2012;153(6):1193–8. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.021.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tolle TR, Treede RD, Beyer A, et al. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006;123(3):231–43. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.01.041.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Geber C, Klein T, Azad S, Birklein F, Gierthmuhlen J, Huge V, et al. Test-retest and interobserver reliability of quantitative sensory testing according to the protocol of the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): a multi-centre study. Pain. 2011;152(3):548–56. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2010.11.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Magerl W, Krumova EK, Baron R, Tolle T, Treede RD, Maier C. Reference data for quantitative sensory testing (QST): refined stratification for age and a novel method for statistical comparison of group data. Pain. 2010;151(3):598–605. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2010.07.026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ziegler EA, Magerl W, Meyer RA, Treede RD. Secondary hyperalgesia to punctate mechanical stimuli. Central sensitization to a-fibre nociceptor input. Brain. 1999;122(Pt 12):2245–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain. 2011;152(3 Suppl):S2–15. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Herrmann C. International experiences with the hospital anxiety and depression scale--a review of validation data and clinical results. J Psychosom Res. 1997;42(1):17–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67(6):361–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Ruscheweyh R, Marziniak M, Stumpenhorst F, Reinholz J, Knecht S. Pain sensitivity can be assessed by self-rating: development and validation of the pain sensitivity questionnaire. Pain. 2009;146(1–2):65–74. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.06.020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Granot M, Granovsky Y, Sprecher E, Nir RR, Yarnitsky D. Contact heat-evoked temporal summation: tonic versus repetitive-phasic stimulation. Pain. 2006;122(3):295–305. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.02.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Hollins M, Harper D, Maixner W. Changes in pain from a repetitive thermal stimulus: the roles of adaptation and sensitization. Pain. 2011;152(7):1583–90. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2011.02.049.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Tesarz J, Gerhardt A, Schommer K, Treede RD, Eich W. Alterations in endogenous pain modulation in endurance athletes: an experimental study using quantitative sensory testing and the cold-pressor task. Pain. 2013;154(7):1022–9. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.03.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Weir JP. Quantifying test-retest reliability using the intraclass correlation coefficient and the SEM. J Strength Cond Res. 2005;19(1):231–40. doi:10.1519/15184.1.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Harvill LM. Standard error of measurement. Educ Meas Issues Pract. 1991;10:33–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Bland JM, Altman DG. Comparing methods of measurement: why plotting difference against standard method is misleading. Lancet. 1995;346(8982):1085–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Grouven U, Bender R, Ziegler A, Lange S. Vergleich von Messmethoden. Dtsch Med Wochenschr. 2007;132:e69–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Julious SA, Campbell MJ. Tutorial in biostatistics: sample sizes for parallel group clinical trials with binary data. Stat Med. 2012;31(24):2904–36. doi:10.1002/sim.5381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Edwards RR, Fillingim RB, Ness TJ. Age-related differences in endogenous pain modulation: a comparison of diffuse noxious inhibitory controls in healthy older and younger adults. Pain. 2003;101(1–2):155–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Quiton RL, Greenspan JD. Sex differences in endogenous pain modulation by distracting and painful conditioning stimulation. Pain. 2007;132 Suppl 1:S134–49. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.09.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Willer JC, De Broucker T, Le Bars D. Encoding of nociceptive thermal stimuli by diffuse noxious inhibitory controls in humans. J Neurophysiol. 1989;62(5):1028–38.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Baad-Hansen L, Poulsen HF, Jensen HM, Svensson P. Lack of sex differences in modulation of experimental intraoral pain by diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC). Pain. 2005;116(3):359–65. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2005.05.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Nahman-Averbuch H, Martucci KT, Granovsky Y, Weissman-Fogel I, Yarnitsky D, Coghill RC. Distinct brain mechanisms support spatial vs temporal filtering of nociceptive information. Pain. 2014;155(12):2491–501. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2014.07.008.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Piche M, Arsenault M, Rainville P. Cerebral and cerebrospinal processes underlying counterirritation analgesia. J Neurosci. 2009;29(45):14236–46. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2341-09.2009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Terkelsen AJ, Andersen OK, Hansen PO, Jensen TS. Effects of heterotopic- and segmental counter-stimulation on the nociceptive withdrawal reflex in humans. Acta Physiol Scand. 2001;172(3):211–7. doi:10.1046/j.1365-201x.2001.00856.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Streff A, Kuehl LK, Michaux G, Anton F. Differential physiological effects during tonic painful hand immersion tests using hot and ice water. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(3):266–72. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.05.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Serrao M, Rossi P, Sandrini G, Parisi L, Amabile GA, Nappi G, et al. Effects of diffuse noxious inhibitory controls on temporal summation of the RIII reflex in humans. Pain. 2004;112(3):353–60. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2004.09.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Pickering G, Pereira B, Dufour E, Soule S, Dubray C. Impaired modulation of pain in patients with postherpetic neuralgia. Pain Res Manag. 2014;19(1):e19–23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Moont R, Pud D, Sprecher E, Sharvit G, Yarnitsky D. ‘Pain inhibits pain’ mechanisms: Is pain modulation simply due to distraction? Pain. 2010;150(1):113–20. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2010.04.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Moont R, Crispel Y, Lev R, Pud D, Yarnitsky D. Temporal changes in cortical activation during distraction from pain: a comparative LORETA study with conditioned pain modulation. Brain Res. 2012;1435:105–17. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.11.056.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Vaegter HB, Handberg G, Graven-Nielsen T. Similarities between exercise-induced hypoalgesia and conditioned pain modulation in humans. Pain. 2014;155(1):158–67. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.09.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Washington LL, Gibson SJ, Helme RD. Age-related differences in the endogenous analgesic response to repeated cold water immersion in human volunteers. Pain. 2000;89(1):89–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Oono Y, Baad-Hansen L, Wang K, Arendt-Nielsen L, Svensson P. Effect of conditioned pain modulation on trigeminal somatosensory function evaluated by quantitative sensory testing. Pain. 2013. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.07.049.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Razavi M, Hansson PT, Johansson B, Leffler AS. The influence of intensity and duration of a painful conditioning stimulation on conditioned pain modulation in volunteers. Eur J Pain. 2013. doi:10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00435.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Klauenberg S, Maier C, Assion HJ, Hoffmann A, Krumova EK, Magerl W, et al. Depression and changed pain perception: hints for a central disinhibition mechanism. Pain. 2008;140(2):332–43. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2008.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Uhl I, Krumova EK, Regeniter S, Bar KJ, Norra C, Richter H, et al. Association between wind-up ratio and central serotonergic function in healthy subjects and depressed patients. Neurosci Lett. 2011;504(2):176–80. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2011.09.033.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Boettger MK, Grossmann D, Bar KJ. Thresholds and perception of cold pain, heat pain, and the thermal grill illusion in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychosom Med. 2013;75(3):281–7. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182881a9c.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Terhaar J, Boettger MK, Schwier C, Wagner G, Israel AK, Bar KJ. Increased sensitivity to heat pain after sad mood induction in female patients with major depression. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(5):559–63. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.09.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Vidor LP, Torres IL, Medeiros LF, Dussan-Sarria JA, Dall’agnol L, Deitos A, et al. Association of anxiety with intracortical inhibition and descending pain modulation in chronic myofascial pain syndrome. BMC Neurosci. 2014;15:42. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-15-42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Ruhe HG, Mason NS, Schene AH. Mood is indirectly related to serotonin, norepinephrine and dopamine levels in humans: a meta-analysis of monoamine depletion studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12(4):331–59. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001949.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Treister R, Pud D, Eisenberg E. The dopamine agonist apomorphine enhances conditioned pain modulation in healthy humans. Neurosci Lett. 2013;548:115–9. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2013.05.041.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Yoshimura M, Furue H. Mechanisms for the anti-nociceptive actions of the descending noradrenergic and serotonergic systems in the spinal cord. J Pharmacol Sci. 2006;101(2):107–17.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D. The validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale. An updated literature review. J Psychosom Res. 2002;52(2):69–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Nir RR, Yarnitsky D, Honigman L, Granot M. Cognitive manipulation targeted at decreasing the conditioning pain perception reduces the efficacy of conditioned pain modulation. Pain. 2012;153(1):170–6. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2011.10.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Grashorn W, Sprenger C, Forkmann K, Wrobel N, Bingel U. Age-dependent decline of endogenous pain control: exploring the effect of expectation and depression. PLoS One. 2013;8(9):e75629. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0075629.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Agostinho CM, Scherens A, Richter H, Schaub C, Rolke R, Treede RD, et al. Habituation and short-term repeatability of thermal testing in healthy human subjects and patients with chronic non-neuropathic pain. Eur J Pain. 2009;13(8):779–85. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.10.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Treister R, Eisenberg E, Gershon E, Haddad M, Pud D. Factors affecting - and relationships between-different modes of endogenous pain modulation in healthy volunteers. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(6):608–14. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.10.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Rezaii T, Hirschberg AL, Carlstrom K, Ernberg M. The influence of menstrual phases on pain modulation in healthy women. J Pain. 2012;13(7):646–55. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2012.04.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are indebted to the subjects who participated in this study for their consent and cooperation.

Funding

There was no specific funding for this study; this work is part of the doctoral thesis of Julia Gehling. Regarding the publication fees, we acknowledge support by the Open Access Publication Funds of the Ruhr-Universität Bochum.

Availability of data and material

The datasets during and/or analysed during the current study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Authors’ contributions

All authors contributed to the conception and design of the study, collaborated on the statistical analysis of the data, discussed the results, participated in writing the manuscript and approved the final version. JG conducted all study experiments.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocol was in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local ethics committee of the Faculty of Medicine, Ruhr-University Bochum, Germany (Reg. Nr. 4321–12, NCT01618604). Before starting the assessment, the study was described in its entirety to the subjects and they all gave their written informed consent to participate.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elena K. Enax-Krumova.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gehling, J., Mainka, T., Vollert, J. et al. Short-term test-retest-reliability of conditioned pain modulation using the cold-heat-pain method in healthy subjects and its correlation to parameters of standardized quantitative sensory testing. BMC Neurol 16, 125 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-016-0650-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-016-0650-z

Keywords