Skip to main content

Disease severity and mortality in Alzheimer's disease: an analysis using the U.S. National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center Uniform Data Set

Abstract

Background

Evidence on the relative risk of death across all stages of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is lacking but greatly needed for the evaluation of new interventions. We used data from the Uniform Data Set (UDS) of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) to assess the expected survival of a person progressing to a particular stage of AD and the relative risk of death for a person in a particular stage of AD compared with cognitively normal (CN) people.

Methods

This was a retrospective observational cohort study of mortality and its determinants in participants with incident mild cognitive impairment (MCI) due to AD or AD dementia compared with CN participants. Overall survival and hazard ratios of all-cause mortality in participants ≥ 50 years of age with clinically assessed or diagnosed MCI due to AD, or mild, moderate, or severe AD dementia, confirmed by Clinical Dementia Rating scores, versus CN participants were estimated, using NACC UDS data. Participants were followed until death, censoring, or until information to determine disease stage was missing.

Results

Aged between 50 and 104 years, 12,414 participants met the eligibility criteria for the study. Participants progressing to MCI due to AD or AD dementia survived a median of 3–12 years, with higher mortality observed in more severe stages. Risk of death increased with the severity of AD dementia, with the increase significantly higher at younger ages. Participants with MCI due to AD and CN participants had a similar risk of death after controlling for confounding factors.

Conclusions

Relative all-cause mortality risk increases with AD severity, more so at younger ages. Mortality does not seem to be higher for those remaining in MCI due to AD. Findings might imply potential benefit of lower mortality if preventing or delaying the progression of AD is successful, and importantly, this potential benefit might be greater in relatively younger people. Future research should replicate our study in other samples more representative of the general US population as well as other populations around the world.

Peer Review reports

Background

The burden of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is enormous, with the latest global estimates suggesting that up to 100 million people could be affected when including those in the early symptomatic stages of the disease (i.e., mild cognitive impairment [MCI] due to AD and AD dementia) [1]. AD is associated with impaired quality of life and premature death [2, 3], substantial burden to family care partners [4], and large costs to society [5]. As global numbers are expected to rise, policymakers and drug developers are determined to find a way to prevent or slow disease progression in AD. Improving our understanding of the relationship between AD and mortality is crucial to achieving this in two key ways. Firstly, mortality is a major concern for people living with AD and their families [6, 7]. Secondly, the relationship between AD and mortality is expected to have a major impact on the long-term consequences and value of efforts to prevent or delay the disease, since treatment benefit would be determined by a therapy’s ability to both reduce morbidity and extend life [8]. At present we have limited knowledge of precisely how mortality is affected by AD, and specifically by AD progression and disease severity. This limits our ability to assess the value of interventions and thus make informed policy decisions.

Dementia is the fifth leading cause of death worldwide, accounting for 2.4 million deaths worldwide in 2016 [3]. The median survival after a dementia diagnosis has been estimated to be between 3 and 7 years [9], suggesting a loss of approximately 9–10 years of life for a 70-year-old person with AD dementia [10]. The available literature consistently demonstrates reductions in life expectancy in AD and dementia, but there are several confounding factors that complicate the picture [9, 11, 12]. In addition to age, key risk factors for death in AD populations include male sex [13,14,15,16,17], advanced disease severity [13,14,15,16,17] and comorbidities (in particular diabetes [13, 14, 18] but also hypertension [18]), coronary artery disease (CAD) [14], and cerebrovascular disease [15, 19]. Additional factors found to be associated with an increased risk of death in some AD studies include being White [15, 20, 21], lower levels of education [17, 19], being underweight (according to body mass index [BMI]) [22], and apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4-positivity (albeit only in men) [23]. Living in an institutional care setting has also been shown to be associated with an increased risk of death in people with dementia of any cause [24].

Evidence on how disease severity impacts the risk of death is essential for disease models used to predict long-term outcomes and evaluate new interventions. Such long-term effects are typically not possible to assess in randomized controlled trials because the length of follow-up is too short; therefore, longer-term observational studies are also needed [8]. There have been a number of such studies in people with AD [13, 14, 25] and all-cause dementia populations [24,25,26] but these can have significant limitations, especially when evaluating interventions in early disease. Firstly, studies on predementia stages of disease are lacking and rarely make comparisons with cognitively normal (CN) participants [13, 14, 24, 25]. Secondly, some studies perform their analyses on prevalent cases [24, 25], thereby lacking a clinically relevant starting point for the exposure and potentially introducing survival bias [12]. Thirdly, some studies define disease stage based on single outcome measures (e.g., cognition) [24], thereby omitting other important symptoms such as functional impairment and behavior. Finally, some studies run their analyses on cohorts combining several dementia etiologies [24, 26] or do not account for important confounding factors (e.g., comorbidities) [25, 26], both of which limit the generalizability of their findings to specific populations.

This study was therefore performed on a well-characterized US population, enabling up to 15 years of follow-up of participants across all clinical stages of AD. We had two main research questions: 1) What is the expected survival of a person progressing to a particular stage of AD? and 2) What is the relative risk of death of a person in a particular stage of AD compared with CN people?

Methods

Study design

This was a retrospective observational cohort study of mortality, and the factors that affect the risk of death, in participants with incident MCI due to AD or AD dementia compared with CN participants. Individual-level data were sourced from the Uniform Data Set (UDS) of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) [27]. Study participants were identified in the NACC UDS at any time during the study period September 1, 2005 to March 1, 2021.

Data sources

The NACC UDS is a large, longitudinal, and well-structured data set of participants with varying cognitive statuses, ranging from normal cognition to MCI and demented. An additional category, “impaired not MCI”, exists for subjects who are cognitively impaired but do not meet the criteria for MCI [27]. Standardized data have been collected prospectively at Alzheimer’s Disease Research Centers (ADRCs) throughout the US since 2005, funded by the National Institute on Aging (NIA). The data used in this study included all visits for the subset of UDS participants who consented to allow the sharing of research data with commercial entities. This analysis used data from 33 ADRCs.

After the initial UDS visit, individuals had an annual follow-up visit until death or dropout. The data collected included socio-demographics, family and health history, neurologic exam findings, functional status, clinical diagnosis, neuropsychologic test results, imaging availability, and APOE status.

Disease severity measures of relevance used in this study included the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR®) Dementia Staging Instrument [28] (providing both Global Scores [CDR-GS] and Sum of Boxes [CDR-SB]), the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [29] and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [30] for cognitive impairment, the Functional Activities Questionnaire (FAQ) [31] for functional impairment, and the Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire (NPI-Q) [32] for behavioral and psychiatric symptoms.

Deaths were reported by the ADRCs via a specific milestones form, and therefore these were only for participants who were actively followed.

Eligibility criteria and cohorts

Eligible participants had an index visit at 50 years old or older and contributed an observation time of at least one day. At their index visit, participants were classified into one of three cohorts: CN, MCI due to AD, and AD dementia; see Additional file 1 for details on variables and cutoff scores for each cohort.

CN participants had a clinical diagnosis of normal cognition confirmed by CDR scores at index, and no prior record of impairment at any pre-index visit. Since clinical diagnosis of normal cognition is based on the absence of symptoms, subjects in this cohort could be in the preclinical stages of AD. Participants with MCI due to AD had a clinical diagnosis of amnestic or non-amnestic MCI confirmed by CDR scores at index, and no prior record of more severe impairment. To limit to incident cases of AD, participants were further required to have a pre-index visit with normal cognition or impaired not MCI, confirmed by CDR scores within 15 months before the index date. They were also required to have a record of AD etiology at index or after, and no record of non-AD etiologic diagnosis potentially causing cognitive impairment before or at index, with the exception of anxiety and depression, because these have been suggested to be possible first signs of AD [33, 34] (see Additional file 1).

Participants with AD dementia had a clinical diagnosis of dementia, confirmed by CDR and MMSE scores at index, and no prior record of such impairment. They were required to have a pre-index visit with normal cognition, impaired not MCI, or MCI, confirmed by CDR scores within 15 months before the index date. They were also required to have a presumptive primary etiologic diagnosis of AD at index, and no record of a non-AD etiologic diagnosis potentially causing cognitive impairment before or at index, with the exception of anxiety and depression. Finally, to enable further dementia staging, participants with AD dementia were required to have complete MMSE or MoCA scores, complete NPI-Q scores, and at least five completed tasks for the FAQ scale at index (see Additional file 1).

Due to limitations in data availability, biomarker confirmation of AD etiology was not possible, except for in a subset of participants with biomarker-positive MCI due to AD (n = 47) who were considered in a sensitivity analysis. Biomarker-positivity was defined as abnormally elevated amyloid on positron emission tomography or abnormally low amyloid in cerebrospinal fluid.

Participants with AD dementia were further classified into one of three disease severity stages (mild, moderate, and severe), according to a previously published model using scores on cognitive, behavioral, and functional symptom domains as measured by the MMSE, NPI-Q, and FAQ scales [35] (see Additional file 1).

Individual participants could contribute observation time to several cohorts upon disease progression if they met the corresponding eligibility criteria. Back-transitions to less severe stages of AD were disregarded to simplify the analysis, and as they are likely temporary in most cases (Supplementary Table 2) [36, 37].

Follow-up

Participants were followed until death, censoring (either at the end of the study period or due to discontinuation or loss to follow-up), or until information to determine disease stage was missing; participants who progressed to a more severe disease stage were included in the subsequent cohort if they met the eligibility criteria (see Additional file 1 and Supplementary Table 1).

Outcomes

Key outcomes included overall survival (median time in years from progression to a particular stage, or study entry for CN participants, to date of death), and hazard ratios (HRs) for different disease stages in comparison with CN participants, with or without adjustment for age, and other potential confounding variables. Date of death was recorded with a precision of year and month in the NACC UDS. For our purpose, the date was assumed to be the last day of each month, thereby avoiding records of visits after the assumed date of death, while acknowledging this could potentially lead to overestimating the overall survival by an average of about half a month.

Predictors and covariates

Age at the start of a particular stage (or study entry for CN) and disease stage (i.e., MCI due to AD, and mild, moderate, or severe AD dementia) were the primary predictors of the analysis, with CN participants as a reference. The selection of additional covariates was guided by a systematic literature review on the determinants of mortality, and they were considered for inclusion if: a) there was convincing evidence of their association with mortality and b) they could be assumed to vary across the different populations that we would like our results to be generalizable to. The following variables were found to meet these criteria: sex [13,14,15,16,17], years of education [17, 19], race/ethnicity [15, 20, 21], BMI [22], APOE status [23], presence or history of CAD [14], presence or history of cerebrovascular disease [15, 19], and type of residence [24] (see Additional file 1). In addition, variables for current smoking status and alcohol abuse were considered as they are known to cause premature death [38, 39]. All covariates were recorded at study entry except for age, disease stage, and type of residence, which varied over time and were updated at progression.

Statistical analysis

To investigate the expected survival of a person progressing to a particular stage of AD, we assessed overall survival separately for each cohort, with Kaplan–Meier estimators reporting median survival time and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) constructed with the Brookmeyer–Crowley method. In this analysis, participants were not censored upon progression but could contribute observation time (and deaths) to multiple cohorts. Therefore, this analysis did not allow for comparison across cohorts but instead shows the expected survival of a person starting in a particular stage of AD irrespective of whether or not they progress. Kaplan–Meier estimators were not stratified by age or other predictors; they describe the overall survival of participants with the age distribution of our sample cohorts.

To assess the relative risk of death of a person in a particular stage of AD compared with CN participants, we fitted Cox proportional-hazards models, adjusting for age and the other covariates. Participants were censored from the stage of AD upon progression, while adding observation time to the new cohort where possible. Five models were considered, including a crude model with disease stage as the only predictor (model 1), a simple model adding age, sex, and a dichotomous variable for up to (or more than) 12 years of education (model 2), an exploratory model including all preselected predictors and covariates (model 3), and an optimized model with covariates selected by the least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (Lasso, model 4), optimized using fivefold nested cross-validation (see Additional file 1) plus sex (based on clinical judgment and interpretability). The fifth model was based on model 4, adding the interaction between disease stage and age because they had large main effects (before fitting the interaction terms) and are of high importance in evaluating the value of a treatment. Other interactions with disease severity were considered including sex, type of residence, years of education, and race/ethnicity, but were not selected either because they were not significant or because the variation in these variables was considered too low. The proportional-hazards model assumption was tested by visual inspection of Schoenfeld residuals. All models were fitted on participants with complete information on the included variables in each particular model, without any imputation.

Sensitivity analyses

We refitted exploratory model 3 excluding comorbidity variables because it is not clear whether they are on the causal pathway from AD to death (i.e., a mediator) or if they are confounding variables, which, if so, should be controlled [40, 41]. Also, to test whether the COVID-19 pandemic influenced our results, we refitted exploratory model 3 on a data set truncated on March 1, 2020, which is when the COVID-19 lockdown began in the US. We also compared the characteristics of participants with MCI due to AD who had a positive biomarker for amyloid-beta (AMYLPET=1 or AMYLCSF=1) with those in the overall MCI due to AD cohort, but the number of participants was too low for a separate analysis of mortality. The characteristics of participants lost to follow-up were compared with those remaining until death, progression, or data cut.

Finally, in order to test the sensitivity to different classifications of disease severity, we reassigned all participants using an alternative model based on the CDR-SB [42,43,44], which set cutoffs for CDR-SB at 0.5–4 for MCI due to AD, 4.5–9 for mild AD dementia, 9.5–15.5 for moderate AD dementia, and 16–18 for severe AD dementia [44]. These updated the CDR-GS eligibility criteria and the multi-domain model for AD dementia stages, whereas other criteria were unchanged (Supplementary Table 3). All Cox proportional-hazards models 1 through 5 were refitted using this alternative classification.

Results

Participant characteristics

The NACC UDS included 33,874 participants with a visit at ≥ 50 years of age, of whom a total of 12,414 participants met the eligibility criteria for at least one of our cohorts (Table 1, Supplementary Table 3). In 1,045 instances, a participant progressed to a more severe stage and could be included in another cohort, and 917 participants died (Table 1). The mean duration of follow-up ranged from about 2–3 years in AD dementia, up to 3.7 years in MCI due to AD, and 4.4 years in CN, while the maximum follow-up was 15 years (Supplementary Table 1). Most participants with severe AD dementia died during follow-up. In contrast, most participants in the other cohorts either progressed or discontinued before the event of death or end of study.

Table 1 Number of participants per cohort, their progressions, deaths, and median Survival (95% CI)

Participants were aged between 50 and 104 years at study entry, with higher mean ages in the AD cohorts compared with CN (Table 2). About two-thirds (65%) were female, and most were non-Hispanic Whites (75%).

Table 2 Participant characteristics by cohort (at start of stage; some participants included in several cohorts if progressing)

Observed survival

The median overall survival of a person progressing to AD dementia or MCI due to AD was estimated at between 3 and 12 years, with higher mortality in more severe stages (Fig. 1, Table 1). The observation period was too short to estimate median survival in CN participants, as well as for an upper bound of the CI for MCI due to AD.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Overall survival of participants progressing to a particular stage of AD. Shaded areas show 95% confidence intervals. Some participants contribute to several stages upon progression. Please note, any comparison across stages underestimates the uncertainty because some participants contribute to several cohorts. The curve of cognitively normal lacks a relevant clinical interpretation as the starting point is their first visit at ≥ 50 years of age. Kaplan–Meier estimators were not stratified by age or other predictors; they describe the overall survival of participants with the age distribution of our sample cohorts. Numbers at risk at 0, 5, 10, and 15 years respectively: CN: 11458, 4185, 1272, 64; MCI due to AD: 937, 249, 50, 0; mild AD dementia: 522, 139, 11, 0; moderate AD dementia: 457, 77, 2, 0; severe AD dementia: 85, 8, 0, 0. AD Alzheimer’s disease, CN cognitively normal, MCI mild cognitive impairment

Relative risk of death across disease stages

The Cox proportional-hazards models showed a clear pattern of increasing mortality with increasing disease severity (Table 3). The effect sizes were attenuated when controlling for potential confounding factors, but remained statistically significant, with the exception of MCI due to AD compared with CN participants. Additional predictors of higher mortality included: higher age, male sex, residential or institutionalized care setting, presence or history of CAD, currently smoking, and alcohol abuse. Weight categories were defined using BMI [45]; being overweight (BMI = 25.0–29.9) but not obese (BMI ≥ 30.0) predicted lower mortality compared with being underweight (BMI < 18.5) or with BMI in the normal range (BMI = 18.5–24.9); Asian participants had lower relative risk of death compared with White participants, and Hispanic/Latino participants compared with non-Hispanic/Latino participants. Education and APOE status were not significant predictors of mortality. Due to missing data, the more comprehensive models were based on fewer observations, while the underlying analysis populations were not substantially different in terms of baseline characteristics (Supplementary Table 8).

Table 3 Relative risks of death estimated by Cox proportional-hazards models

Of note, there was a statistically significant interaction between age and disease severity, suggesting that the relative effect on mortality from increasing disease severity is higher in younger participants (Fig. 2). For instance, the HRs (95% CI) for mortality at 65 years of age were 6.7 (2.7–16.9), 14.8 (8.8–24.8), and 30.1 (16.7–56.8) for mild, moderate, and severe AD, respectively; at 80 years of age, the corresponding HRs (95% CI) were 2.4 (1.7–3.3), 3.1 (2.4–3.9), and 6.6 (4.8–9.1), respectively (Supplementary Table 4).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Hazard ratios (filled lines) with 95% confidence intervals (dashed lines) of risk of death compared with cognitively normal participants by disease stage (panels) and age (x-axis). AD Alzheimer’s disease

Sensitivity analyses

The two sensitivity analyses on the exploratory model 3, i.e., removing comorbidity predictors and limiting data to pre-COVID-19 lockdown, only showed minor impacts on the results (Supplementary Table 5). Participants with MCI due to AD with a positive AD biomarker (n = 47) were similar to the overall MCI due to AD cohort (Supplementary Table 6), but there were too few participants for a separate analysis. Back-transitions to less-severe stages did occur, but in a minority of cases (Supplementary Table 2). Participants lost to follow-up were somewhat younger, more often females, and, on average, had fewer symptoms (Supplementary Table 9). This may be expected because these factors reduce the risk of death and, with regard to age and symptoms, also progression, which are competing risks of loss to follow-up.

The alternative classification model of disease severity based on CDR-SB resulted in slightly different estimates, but did not change the direction of any effects and resulted in marginal changes to the significance of some predictors (Supplementary Tables 7 and 4). Overall, the relative mortality risks in AD dementia stages were somewhat higher with the CDR-SB model compared with the multi-domain model. Notably, the number of participants with CDR-SB scores available (n = 12,448) was lower than those included in the multi-domain model (n = 13,459), and especially low in the AD dementia group.

Discussion

Main findings

In this study, we show that all-cause mortality increases with the severity of symptoms in AD dementia, including when controlling for other predictors of mortality. We also show that this relative increase in the risk for death is higher in younger people. This is supported by previous studies [12] but to our knowledge has not been considered in earlier reports on relative risks. It implies that we can expect increased longevity from AD prevention interventions, and importantly, that this potential benefit is greater in relatively younger people. For example, according to our estimates, an 80-year-old person with severe AD dementia has a sevenfold risk of dying compared with an 80-year-old CN person, whereas the corresponding risk increase for a 65-year-old person with severe AD dementia is 31 times higher (albeit with a wide CI ranging from 16.7 to 56.8, suggesting inadequate power and lower estimation precision). The relatively lower impact on mortality from AD in advanced ages is likely due to competing causes of death. It should be noted that specific causes of death are not captured in the NACC UDS and are therefore not considered in this analysis.

We found no increase in mortality associated with MCI due to AD after controlling for confounding factors and, importantly, disease progression over time. People in the early stages of AD should indeed be expected to have a shorter survival than CN persons, because most will progress to AD dementia over time [46] and AD dementia clearly causes increased mortality. This has been demonstrated by previous studies albeit in persons with MCI due to any cause [47,48,49]; however, our analysis further suggests that there is no significant increase in mortality for people while remaining in the MCI due to AD stage. This does not mean that they will not have an increased risk of dying later, which, again, is consistent with previous studies. In our Cox proportional-hazards models, participants with MCI due to AD who progressed to AD dementia were censored from this particular cohort at the time of progression. However, we cannot assume that these censored participants would have the same mortality risk going forward as compared with those remaining in the MCI due to AD stage. The Kaplan–Meier estimates on unadjusted and non-censored data were also consistent with this interpretation. The higher death rate in people with MCI due to AD compared with CN people may be explained by more advanced age and relatively more men in the MCI due to AD stage, in addition to this population having a higher rate of progression to AD dementia, which is associated with higher mortality. However, there is a possibility that our methods prevented us from finding an actual difference in mortality, as well as in MCI due to AD. Our eligibility criteria were somewhat more restrictive for AD cohorts compared with CN participants, as we excluded participants with a record of non-AD etiologic diagnosis potentially causing cognitive impairment before or at index. This may have resulted in underestimation of the relative mortality in MCI due to AD compared with CN participants.

Overall, our findings on increasing mortality with increasing severity of dementia are consistent with previous studies [13, 14, 24,25,26], albeit with varying effect sizes, probably largely explained by differences in the source populations, definitions of disease stages, and methodologies. For example, the HRs reported by Andersen et al. [26] from a Danish cohort of participants with prevalent and incident dementia were approximately 3, 4, and 10 for mild, moderate, and severe dementia, respectively, defined by CDR and after adjusting for age and sex. These estimates were quite close to the estimates from our sensitivity analysis using CDR-SB (model 2 resulted in HRs 2, 4, and 9 for mild, moderate, and severe AD dementia, respectively). Also, consistent with previous studies, we found that male sex [13,14,15,16,17, 47] and residential or institutionalized care setting are important predictors of increased mortality [24]. Other significant factors that emerged included presence or history of CAD, currently smoking, alcohol abuse, being overweight, Asian race, and Hispanic ethnicity (levels of education and APOE status were not significant factors). Moreover, we should expect collinearity between several variables, which may imply that the effect of some predictors of mortality is nullified when controlling for others; however, we employed methodology (i.e., Lasso) to try to account for such a limitation.

This study did not and could not assess the causality of AD progression on mortality. This potentially causal effect needs to be tested in experimental studies, long enough to show an impact of AD prevention on the mortality outcome. Although many studies have shown that AD is associated with premature death, this association does not necessarily need to be constant when modifying the progression of the disease. In other words, delaying disease progression, as measured by severity of symptoms or AD biomarkers, does not necessarily imply increased longevity, although this is highly likely.

Strengths

One strength of this study was the long follow-up (up to 15 years) on a large (n = 12,414) and well-characterized study population, encompassing all clinical stages of AD and CN participants. This enabled us to consider incident cases of AD and study them from the start of any particular clinical disease stage to progression or death. Another strength was the availability of annual assessments across all relevant symptom domains, including cognition, function, and behavior, which allowed us to classify participants into disease stages according to a comprehensive multi-domain disease model, and not just by MMSE scores. Furthermore, the availability of data on etiology (albeit limited biomarker data), comorbidities, and other characteristics allowed us to control for potential confounding factors for mortality. The resulting adjusted models increased the generalizability of our findings to populations that differ from the NACC UDS, e.g., in terms of age or sex.

Limitations

Our study had several limitations. Firstly, the NACC UDS is a volunteer-based case series data set and is not considered a representative sample of the general US population and therefore includes limited variation between some variables. For instance, participants are more highly educated and non-Hispanic White persons are over-represented compared with the general US population limiting the study’s ability to draw conclusions about mortality risks in different racial and ethnic groups, and thus in the overall US population [50]. Non-Hispanic White persons represent approximately 71% of all participants in the NACC UDS [51], compared with 59% of the general US population [52]. The NACC UDS population is also more highly educated and women are slightly over-represented (57% in the NACC UDS vs 51% of the general US population). Comparison of our data to life tables suggests that our cohort may have longer average life expectancy than the general US population. As an example, a subset of CN participants (414 women and 287 men) aged 85 years and above had a median survival of 9.4 and 7.2 years, respectively, which is higher than the expected survival in the US of 85 years of age: median survival 7.1 years for women and 6.0 years for men [53]. The difference may be explained by the inclusion of people with dementia in the latter estimate, which should result in lower overall survival. However, the selection of participants into the NACC UDS may have caused us to underestimate the mortality in CN participants and therefore overestimate the relative risk of death in AD.

Secondly, there is a risk of underreporting of mortality because the NACC UDS relied on ADRCs to report deaths and data were therefore only available for actively followed participants. This problem was mitigated by censoring participants without a recent follow-up, but this may have instead increased the risk of informative censoring, i.e., if participants with a higher/lower mortality have a higher risk of being lost to follow-up. This may have biased our HRs if censoring was more common in either of the cohorts. Examining the attrition rates (Supplementary Table 1), we noticed some variation across cohorts. However, censoring due to loss to follow-up was most common in the CN and MCI due to AD cohorts (as well as moderate AD dementia), in contrast to what would be expected if increased disease severity increases the risk of discontinuation. In fact, there was no consistent association between stage and percentage lost to follow-up. Furthermore, participants who were lost to follow-up were similar to those who were not, and, if anything, were slightly younger, more often female, and with fewer symptoms overall. Both these findings may suggest that discontinuation is not primarily associated with increased disease severity or risk of death, and therefore a lower risk of informative censoring. However, acknowledging there is still a risk of bias due to informative censoring in our analysis; this most likely should have caused us to underestimate the relative risk of death in AD, at least if relatively healthy participants in each cohort had a higher likelihood of continuing.

Thirdly, there is a risk of misclassification bias, both in terms of etiology of symptoms and staging of disease. Biomarker data were only available in a small subset of participants and could therefore not be used to confirm AD etiology in participants with a clinical AD diagnosis. This may have caused us to misclassify both AD and non-AD participants across all cohorts; especially in MCI due to AD where symptoms are subtle. Encouragingly, in a previous NACC study, investigators reported that 438 (83.3%) of 526 participants with clinically probable AD actually met neuropathologic criteria for AD [54]. Also, baseline characteristics of participants with biomarker-confirmed MCI due to AD were similar to those of participants in the MCI due to AD cohort (Supplementary Table 6). Similarly, the staging of disease severity is dependent on the accuracy of individual tests and the scoring algorithm to assign participants to different stages. Cognitive measurements are imprecise and may fluctuate over time [36]. This is seen in back-transitions to less-severe stages (Supplementary Table 2). We chose to disregard back-transitions as they are likely temporary in most cases. In a previous study, 38% of MCI participants reverted to CN, but 65% subsequently developed MCI or dementia, suggesting that a diagnosis of MCI at any time, even among participants who revert to CN, has prognostic value for AD dementia [37]. Our sensitivity analysis showed that a different algorithm, classifying people into severity stages by CDR-SB, had some impact on the HRs (Supplementary Table 7). There is no prevailing standard for the definition of disease stages, and it is therefore important to make sure that disease stages are defined consistently across combined data sets and models. Other factors that may have caused misclassification include changing criteria for diagnosis across the NACC UDS versions (National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Diseases and Stroke/Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association [NINCDS/ADRDA] in version 1 [55] and version 2 [56] and NIA – Alzheimer's Association [NIA-AA] in version 3 [57]), as well as potential reporting biases (e.g., via participant health history) and missingness in variables used to determine comorbidity.

A fourth noteworthy, but probably marginal, limitation is related to the lack of precision in the timing of the data records. Death was recorded by year and month in the NACC UDS, with the death date set to the last day of the month to avoid negative follow-up time. This should have resulted in an average overestimation of survival across all groups of about half a month. Similarly, events of progression were not continuously recorded (as per other AD studies, to our knowledge), which implies that participants may have progressed earlier than at the visit when progression was assessed. Neither of these limitations should have had any meaningful impact on our results.

Taken together, these limitations should cancel each other out to some degree but there remains a risk of us over- or underestimating the true increase in the risk of death in AD.

Conclusions

We conclude three important findings from this study. Firstly, our data show a relative increase in the risk of death in people in more severe stages of AD. Secondly, this effect is higher in relatively younger persons. Thirdly, our data suggest no increased mortality in persons with MCI due to AD while remaining in this disease stage.

These findings have two major implications. They show that there is a potential benefit of decreased mortality if preventing or delaying the progression of AD is successful, and this benefit is greatest in relatively younger persons and as long as they remain in the MCI due to AD stage. Also, they show the importance of considering both age and disease stage, as well as their interaction, when evaluating health campaigns, prevention strategies, and treatment interventions. The HRs from our Cox proportional-hazards models can be used for this purpose.

In future research, our results should be replicated in other samples more generalizable to the general US population as well as other populations around the world, preferably with linkage to mortality/vital records for more complete reporting of death, and more extensive biomarker-based characterization of persons with AD. Ultimately, clinical trials are necessary to more fully assess the potential causal effect of preventing or delaying the progression of AD on mortality.

Availability of data and materials

Individual-level data were sourced from the Uniform Data Set of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center. Information about the data used in this study, including detailed descriptions and the process for obtaining them, is available at https://naccdata.org/.

Abbreviations

AD: :

Alzheimer’s disease

ADRC: :

Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center

APOE: :

Apolipoprotein E

BMI: :

Body mass index

CAD: :

Coronary artery disease

CDR-GS:

Clinical Dementia Rating – Global Score

CDR-SB:

Clinical Dementia Rating – Sum of Boxes

CI: :

Confidence interval

CN: :

Cognitively normal

FAQ: :

Functional Activities Questionnaire

HR: :

Hazard ratio

MCI: :

Mild cognitive impairment

MMSE: :

Mini-Mental State Examination

MoCA: :

Montreal Cognitive Assessment

NACC: :

National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center

NIA: :

National Institute on Aging

NIA-AA: :

National Institute on Aging – Alzheimer's Association

NINCDS/ADRDA: :

National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Diseases and Stroke/Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association

NPI-Q: :

Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire

UDS: :

Uniform Data Set

References

  1. Gustavsson A, Norton N, Fast T, Frolich L, Georges J, et al. Global estimates on the number of persons across the Alzheimer’s disease continuum. Alzheimers Dement. 2023;19:658–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Gustavsson A, Raket LL, Lilja M, Rutten-Jacobs L, Fues Wahl H, et al. Health utility in preclinical and prodromal Alzheimer’s disease for establishing the value of new disease-modifying treatments-EQ-5D data from the Swedish BioFINDER study. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:1832–42.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. GBD Dementia Collaborators. Global, regional, and national burden of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18:88–106.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Brodaty H, Donkin M. Family caregivers of people with dementia. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009;11:217–28.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. WHO. Global status report on the public health response to dementia. Geneva: World Health Organization,. 2021.

  6. Tochel C, Smith M, Baldwin H, Gustavsson A, Ly A, et al. What outcomes are important to patients with mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer’s disease, their caregivers, and health-care professionals? A systematic review. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2019;11:231–47.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Janssen O, Vos SJB, Garcia-Negredo G, Tochel C, Gustavsson A, et al. Real-world evidence in Alzheimer’s disease: the ROADMAP Data Cube. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:461–71.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Gustavsson A, Green C, Jones RW, Forstl H, Simsek D, et al. Current issues and future research priorities for health economic modelling across the full continuum of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13:312–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Todd S, Barr S, Roberts M, Passmore AP. Survival in dementia and predictors of mortality: a review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;28:1109–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Strand BH, Knapskog AB, Persson K, Edwin TH, Amland R, et al. Survival and years of life lost in various aetiologies of dementia, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and subjective cognitive decline (SCD) in Norway. PLoS One. 2018;13: e0204436.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Guehne U, Riedel-Heller S, Angermeyer MC. Mortality in dementia. Neuroepidemiology. 2005;25:153–62.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Brodaty H, Seeher K, Gibson L. Dementia time to death: a systematic literature review on survival time and years of life lost in people with dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 2012;24:1034–45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Go SM, Lee KS, Seo SW, Chin J, Kang SJ, et al. Survival of Alzheimer’s disease patients in Korea. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2013;35:219–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Larson EB, Shadlen MF, Wang L, McCormick WC, Bowen JD, et al. Survival after initial diagnosis of Alzheimer disease. Ann Intern Med. 2004;140:501–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Mueller C, Huntley J, Stubbs B, Sommerlad A, Carvalho AF, et al. Associations of neuropsychiatric symptoms and antidepressant prescription with survival in Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2017;18:1076–81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Ueki A, Shinjo H, Shimode H, Nakajima T, Morita Y. Factors associated with mortality in patients with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease: a five-year longitudinal study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16:810–5.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Liang FW, Chan W, Chen PJ, Zimmerman C, Waring S, et al. Cognitively-related basic activities of daily living impairment greatly increases the risk of death in Alzheimers disease. PLoS One. 2016;11: e0160671.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Helzner EP, Scarmeas N, Cosentino S, Tang MX, Schupf N, et al. Survival in Alzheimer disease: a multiethnic, population-based study of incident cases. Neurology. 2008;71:1489–95.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Xue H, Sun Q, Liu L, Zhou L, Liang R, et al. Risk factors of transition from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease and death: a cohort study. Compr Psychiatry. 2017;78:91–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Cosentino S, Scarmeas N, Albert SM, Stern Y. Verbal fluency predicts mortality in Alzheimer disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2006;19:123–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Mehta KM, Yaffe K, Perez-Stable EJ, Stewart A, Barnes D, et al. Race/ethnic differences in AD survival in US Alzheimer’s Disease Centers. Neurology. 2008;70:1163–70.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Jang H, Kim JH, Choi SH, Lee Y, Hong CH, et al. Body mass index and mortality rate in Korean patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;46:399–406.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Dal Forno G, Carson KA, Brookmeyer R, Troncoso J, Kawas CH, et al. APOE genotype and survival in men and women with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 2002;58:1045–50.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Garcia-Ptacek S, Farahmand B, Kareholt I, Religa D, Cuadrado ML, et al. Mortality risk after dementia diagnosis by dementia type and underlying factors: a cohort of 15,209 patients based on the Swedish Dementia Registry. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;41:467–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Brehaut JC, Raina P, Lindsay J. Does cognitive status modify the relationship between education and mortality? Evidence from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Int Psychogeriatr. 2004;16:75–91.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Andersen K, Lolk A, Martinussen T, Kragh-Sørensen P. Very mild to severe dementia and mortality: a 14-year follow-up - the Odense study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;29:61–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Beekly DL, Ramos EM, Lee WW, Deitrich WD, Jacka ME, et al. The National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) database: the Uniform Data Set. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2007;21:249–58.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43:2412–4.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189-98.

  30. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:695–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Pfeffer RI, Kurosaki TT, Harrah CH Jr, Chance JM, Filos S. Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. J Gerontol. 1982;37:323–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kaufer DI, Cummings JL, Ketchel P, Smith V, MacMillan A, et al. Validation of the NPI-Q, a brief clinical form of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2000;12:233–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Dafsari FS, Jessen F. Depression-an underrecognized target for prevention of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:160.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Hanseeuw BJ, Jonas V, Jackson J, Betensky RA, Rentz DM, et al. Association of anxiety with subcortical amyloidosis in cognitively normal older adults. Mol Psychiatry. 2020;25:2599–607.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Green C, Zhang S. Predicting the progression of Alzheimer’s disease dementia: a multidomain health policy model. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12:776–85.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Lee DR, Taylor JP, Thomas AJ. Assessment of cognitive fluctuation in dementia: a systematic review of the literature. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;27:989–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Mielke MM, Cha RH, Pankratz VS, et al. Higher risk of progression to dementia in mild cognitive impairment cases who revert to normal. Neurology. 2014;82:317–25.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Ferrari P, Licaj I, Muller DC, Kragh Andersen P, Johansson M, et al. Lifetime alcohol use and overall and cause-specific mortality in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and nutrition (EPIC) study. BMJ Open. 2014;4: e005245.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Jacobs DR Jr, Adachi H, Mulder I, Kromhout D, Menotti A, et al. Cigarette smoking and mortality risk: twenty-five-year follow-up of the Seven Countries Study. Arch Intern Med. 1999;159:733–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Fabbri E, Zoli M, Gonzalez-Freire M, Salive ME, Studenski SA, et al. Aging and multimorbidity: new tasks, priorities, and frontiers for integrated gerontological and clinical research. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2015;16:640–7.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Grande G, Marengoni A, Vetrano DL, Roso-Llorach A, Rizzuto D, et al. Multimorbidity burden and dementia risk in older adults: the role of inflammation and genetics. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:768–76.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Herring WL, Gould IG, Fillit H, Lindgren P, Forrestal F, et al. Predicted lifetime health outcomes for aducanumab in patients with early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurol Ther. 2021;10:919–40.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Potashman M, Buessing M, LevitchiBenea M, Cummings J, Borson S, et al. Estimating progression rates across the spectrum of Alzheimer’s disease for amyloid-positive individuals using National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center data. Neurol Ther. 2021;10:941–53.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. O’Bryant SE, Waring SC, Cullum CM, Hall J, Lacritz L, et al. Staging dementia using Clinical Dementia Rating Scale Sum of Boxes scores: a Texas Alzheimer’s research consortium study. Arch Neurol. 2008;65:1091–5.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC): Defining Adult Overweight & Obesity. https://www.cdc.gov/obesity/basics/adult-defining.html. Accessed 11 Apr 2023.

  46. Vos SJ, Verhey F, Frölich L, Kornhuber J, Wiltfang J, et al. Prevalence and prognosis of Alzheimer’s disease at the mild cognitive impairment stage. Brain. 2015;138:1327–38.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Vassilaki M, Cha RH, Aakre JA, Therneau TM, Geda YE, et al. Mortality in mild cognitive impairment varies by subtype, sex, and lifestyle factors: the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;45:1237–45.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Bae JB, Han JW, Kwak KP, Kim BJ, Kim SG, et al. Impact of mild cognitive impairment on mortality and cause of death in the elderly. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;64:607–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Contador I, Bermejo-Pareja F, Mitchell AJ, Trincado R, Villarejo A, et al. Cause of death in mild cognitive impairment: a prospective study (NEDICES). Eur J Neurol. 2014;21:253-e9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Weintraub S, Salmon D, Mercaldo N, Ferris S, Graff-Radford NR, et al. The Alzheimer’s Disease Centers’ Uniform Data Set (UDS): the neuropsychologic test battery. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2009;23:91–101.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC): Uniform Data Set (UDS): UDS Demographics and diagnoses. https://naccdata.org/requesting-data/data-summary/uds. Accessed 11 Apr 2023.

  52. United States Census Bureau: QuickFacts United States. https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/fact/table/US/EDU635220. Accessed 11 Apr 2023.

  53. Arias E, Xu J. United States Life Tables, 2019. Natl Vital Stat Rep. 2022;70:1–59.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Beach TG, Monsell SE, Phillips LE, Kukull W. Accuracy of the clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease at National Institute on Aging Alzheimer Disease Centers, 2005–2010. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2012;71:266–73.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–44.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:263–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Jack CR Jr, Albert MS, Knopman DS, McKhann GM, Sperling RA, et al. Introduction to the recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:257–62.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The NACC database is funded by NIA/NIH Grant U24 AG072122. NACC data are contributed by the NIA-funded ADCs: P50 AG005131 (PI James Brewer, MD, PhD), P50 AG005133 (PI Oscar Lopez, MD), P50 AG005134 (PI Bradley Hyman, MD, PhD), P50 AG005136 (PI Thomas Grabowski, MD), P50 AG005138 (PI Mary Sano, PhD), P50 AG005142 (PI Helena Chui, MD), P50 AG005146 (PI Marilyn Albert, PhD), P50 AG005681 (PI John Morris, MD), P30 AG008017 (PI Jeffrey Kaye, MD), P30 AG008051 (PI Thomas Wisniewski, MD), P50 AG008702 (PI Scott Small, MD), P30 AG010124 (PI John Trojanowski, MD, PhD), P30 AG010129 (PI Charles DeCarli, MD), P30 AG010133 (PI Andrew Saykin, PsyD), P30 AG010161 (PI David Bennett, MD), P30 AG012300 (PI Roger Rosenberg, MD), P30 AG013846 (PI Neil Kowall, MD), P30 AG013854 (PI Robert Vassar, PhD), P50 AG016573 (PI Frank LaFerla, PhD), P50 AG016574 (PI Ronald Petersen, MD, PhD), P30 AG019610 (PI Eric Reiman, MD), P50 AG023501 (PI Bruce Miller, MD), P50 AG025688 (PI Allan Levey, MD, PhD), P30 AG028383 (PI Linda Van Eldik, PhD), P50 AG033514 (PI Sanjay Asthana, MD, FRCP), P30 AG035982 (PI Russell Swerdlow, MD), P50 AG047266 (PI Todd Golde, MD, PhD), P50 AG047270 (PI Stephen Strittmatter, MD, PhD), P50 AG047366 (PI Victor Henderson, MD, MS), P30 AG049638 (PI Suzanne Craft, PhD), P30 AG053760 (PI Henry Paulson, MD, PhD), P30 AG066546 (PI Sudha Seshadri, MD), P20 AG068024 (PI Erik Roberson, MD, PhD), P20 AG068053 (PI Marwan Sabbagh, MD), P20 AG068077 (PI Gary Rosenberg, MD), P20 AG068082 (PI Angela Jefferson, PhD), P30 AG072958 (PI Heather Whitson, MD), P30 AG072959 (PI James Leverenz, MD).

VC, AR, SQZ, MV, SG, and AG made substantial contributions to the conception, design, analysis, and interpretation of the work. All authors contributed to revising the work critically for important intellectual content and approved the final version to be published.

Funding

This study on secondary data from the NACC UDS was funded by F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd, Basel, Switzerland.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

VC, AR, SQZ, MV, SG, and AG made substantial contributions to the conception, design, analysis, and interpretation of the work. All authors contributed to revising the work critically for important intellectual content and approved the final version to be published.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Valerie Crowell.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Research using the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center database was approved by the University of Washington Institutional Review Board. Informed consent was obtained from all participants at the individual US Alzheimer’s Disease Centers. The National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center data were de-identified. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

VC, SG, and AR are employees and shareholders of F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd. SG is a shareholder of Novartis AG. SZ is employed by Genesis Research, the recipient of consulting fees from F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd. MV has received research funding from F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd and Biogen in past and consults for F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd; she receives research funding from NIH and has equity ownership in Abbott Laboratories, Johnson & Johnson, Medtronic, Merck, and Amgen. AG is a partner of Quantify Research, providing consultancy services to pharmaceutical companies, as well as other private and public organizations and institutions.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Crowell, V., Reyes, A., Zhou, S.Q. et al. Disease severity and mortality in Alzheimer's disease: an analysis using the U.S. National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center Uniform Data Set. BMC Neurol 23, 302 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-023-03353-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-023-03353-w

Keywords